Lateralization and behavioral correlation of changes in regional cerebral blood flow with classical conditioning of the human eyeblink response.

Laterality of changes in regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) during classical conditioning of the human eyeblink response was studied and changes in rCBF were correlated with conditioned responses. In 10 normal volunteers, rCBF was mapped with positron emission tomography and H2(15)O during pairings of a binaural tone conditioned stimulus and an air puff unconditioned stimulus to the left eye. Control conditions consisted of explicitly unpaired presentations of the tone and air puff before (control) and after (extinction) pairings. During pairings, rCBF increased significantly in right primary auditory cortex (contralateral to air puff) and decreased significantly in left and right cerebellar cortex. There were also increases in rCBF in right auditory association cortex and left temporoccipital cortex. Decreases in rCBF were noted bilaterally in the temporal poles and in the left prefrontal cortex. Positive correlations between changes in rCBF and percent conditioned responses were located in middle cerebellum, right superior temporal cortex, left dorsal premotor cortex, right middle cingulate, and right superior temporal cortex. There were negative correlations in left inferior prefrontal cortex, left middle prefrontal cortex, and right inferior parietal cortex. The data replicate our previous findings of lateralized changes in rCBF following presentations of a binaural tone and air puff to the right eye and indicate that there are pairing-specific changes in primary auditory cortex and cerebellum that are not unique to the left or right hemisphere but are a function of the side of training. The commonalities as well as differences in regional involvement in our present and previous experiment as well as in other eyeblink studies illustrate the advantage of functional neuroimaging to quantify different strategies used by the brain to perform seemingly similar functions. Indeed, the data support the notion that learning-related changes can be detected in a number of specific, but not necessarily invariant, brain regions, and that the involvement of any one region is dependent on the characteristics of the particular learning situation.

[1]  E. Hilgard,et al.  The course of acquisition and retention of conditioned eyelid responses in man , 1936 .

[2]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Comparing Functional (PET) Images: The Assessment of Significant Change , 1991, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[3]  B. Schreurs,et al.  A functional anatomical study of associative learning in humans. , 1994, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[4]  N Birbaumer,et al.  Classical conditioning after cerebellar lesions in humans. , 1993, Behavioral neuroscience.

[5]  F. Bookstein,et al.  Developmental Research in Behavioral Teratology: Effects of Prenatal Alcohol Exposure on Child Development , 1994 .

[6]  M. Gluck,et al.  Context, conditioning, and hippocampal rerepresentation in animal learning. , 1994, Behavioral neuroscience.

[7]  E. Pedhazur Multiple Regression in Behavioral Research: Explanation and Prediction , 1982 .

[8]  M. Bouton Context, time, and memory retrieval in the interference paradigms of Pavlovian learning. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.

[9]  N C Andreasen,et al.  A comparison of approaches to the statistical analysis of [15O]H2O PET cognitive activation studies. , 1995, The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[10]  Y. Miyashita Inferior temporal cortex: where visual perception meets memory. , 1993, Annual review of neuroscience.

[11]  T. Cizadlo,et al.  Short-term and long-term verbal memory: a positron emission tomography study. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[12]  Henning Scheich,et al.  Neural substrates for tone-conditioned bradycardia demonstrated with 2-deoxyglucose. II. Auditory cortex plasticity , 1986, Behavioural Brain Research.

[13]  Norman M. Weinberger,et al.  Classical conditioning induces CS-specific receptive field plasticity in the auditory cortex of the guinea pig , 1990, Brain Research.

[14]  M. Merzenich,et al.  Plasticity in the frequency representation of primary auditory cortex following discrimination training in adult owl monkeys , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[15]  D. Pandya,et al.  Prefrontal cortex in relation to other cortical areas in rhesus monkey: architecture and connections. , 1990, Progress in brain research.

[16]  M. Raichle,et al.  The anterior cingulate cortex mediates processing selection in the Stroop attentional conflict paradigm. , 1990, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[17]  F. Gonzalez-Lima,et al.  Brain Imaging of Auditory Learning Functions in Rats: Studies with Fluorodeoxyglucose Autoradiography and Cytochrome Oxidase Histochemistry , 1992 .

[18]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  Functional Mapping of Human Learning: A Positron Emission Tomography Activation Study of Eyeblink Conditioning , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  Fred L. Bookstein,et al.  The enduring effects of prenatal alcohol exposure on child development: Birth through seven years, a partial least squares solution. , 1993 .

[20]  Cheryl L. Grady,et al.  Hemispheric differences in neural systems for face working memory: A PET‐rCBF study , 1995 .

[21]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Localisation in PET Images: Direct Fitting of the Intercommissural (AC—PC) Line , 1989, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[22]  R. F. Thompson,et al.  Organization of memory traces in the mammalian brain. , 1994, Annual review of neuroscience.

[23]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Rapid Automated Algorithm for Aligning and Reslicing PET Images , 1992, Journal of computer assisted tomography.

[24]  D. Perrett,et al.  Visual attention in the occipitotemporal processing stream of the macaque , 1994 .

[25]  G. Lynch,et al.  The neurobiology of learning and memory , 1989, Cognition.

[26]  A. McIntosh,et al.  Network interactions among limbic cortices, basal forebrain, and cerebellum differentiate a tone conditioned as a Pavlovian excitor or inhibitor: fluorodeoxyglucose mapping and covariance structural modeling. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  Henning Scheich,et al.  Learning-Related Plasticity of Gerbil Auditory Cortex: Feature Maps Versus Meaning Maps , 1992 .

[28]  B. J. Casey,et al.  Regional brain activity when selecting a response despite interference: An H2 15O PET study of the stroop and an emotional stroop , 1994, Human brain mapping.

[29]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Functional anatomy of human eyeblink conditioning determined with regional cerebral glucose metabolism and positron-emission tomography. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[30]  Fred L. Bookstein,et al.  Exploiting Redundant Measurement of Dose and Developmental Outcome: New Methods from the Behavioral Teratology of Alcohol. , 1996 .

[31]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The Relationship between Global and Local Changes in PET Scans , 1990, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[32]  M. Hallett,et al.  Deficit in classical conditioning in patients with cerebellar degeneration. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[33]  M. Petrides Deficits on conditional associative-learning tasks after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man , 1985, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  F. Gonzalez-Lima,et al.  Structural equation modeling and its application to network analysis in functional brain imaging , 1994 .

[35]  D. Alkon Learning in a marine snail. , 1983, Scientific American.

[36]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Functional Connectivity: The Principal-Component Analysis of Large (PET) Data Sets , 1993, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[37]  Fred L. Bookstein,et al.  Measuring “dose” and “response” with multtvariate data using partial least squares techniques , 1990 .

[38]  M. Gabriel,et al.  Functions of anterior and posterior cingulate cortex during avoidance learning in rabbits. , 1990, Progress in brain research.

[39]  J. Harvey,et al.  Asymmetric uptake of 2-deoxy-d-[14C]glucose in the dorsal cochlear nucleus during Pavlovian conditioning in the rabbit , 1988, Brain Research.

[40]  J. V. Haxby,et al.  Spatial Pattern Analysis of Functional Brain Images Using Partial Least Squares , 1996, NeuroImage.