Chapter Five – The World from a Dog’s Point of View: A Review and Synthesis of Dog Cognition Research
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Joseph Terkel,et al. Domination of olfaction over vision in explosives detection by dogs , 2003 .
[2] Heini Törnqvist,et al. Dogs do look at images: eye tracking in canine cognition research , 2012, Animal Cognition.
[3] Ádám Miklósi,et al. Social learning in dogs: the effect of a human demonstrator on the performance of dogs in a detour task , 2001, Animal Behaviour.
[4] F. Galibert,et al. Toward understanding dog evolutionary and domestication history. , 2011, Comptes rendus biologies.
[5] Mindreading in a dog: an adaptation of a primate ‘mental attribution’ study , 2006 .
[6] Á. Miklósi,et al. A comparative approach to dogs’ (Canis familiaris) and human infants’ comprehension of various forms of pointing gestures , 2009, Animal Cognition.
[7] W. Roberts,et al. Theory of mind in dogs: is the perspective-taking task a good test? , 2011, Learning & behavior.
[8] P. Kuśmierek,et al. Effect of experimental setting on learning and performance of Auditory Delayed Matching-to-Sample task in dogs. , 1998, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.
[9] J. V. Rooijen. Do dogs and bees possess a ‘theory of mind’? , 2010, Animal Behaviour.
[10] J. E. Rice,et al. Multiple and ancient origins of the domestic dog. , 1997, Science.
[11] Á. Miklósi,et al. Use of experimenter-given cues in dogs , 1998, Animal Cognition.
[12] Márta Gácsi,et al. Provided for Non-commercial Research and Educational Use Only. Not for Reproduction, Distribution or Commercial Use. the Dog as a Model for Understanding Human Social Behavior , 2022 .
[13] Márta Gácsi,et al. Comprehension of human pointing gestures in young human-reared wolves (Canis lupus) and dogs (Canis familiaris) , 2008, Animal Cognition.
[14] C. Wynne,et al. A review of domestic dogs' (Canis familiaris) human-like behaviors: or why behavior analysts should stop worrying and love their dogs. , 2008, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[15] Á. Miklósi,et al. Dogs discriminate between barks: The effect of context and identity of the caller , 2009, Behavioural Processes.
[16] Holly C. Miller,et al. The breakfast effect: Dogs (Canis familiaris) search more accurately when they are less hungry , 2012, Behavioural Processes.
[17] F. Doré,et al. Psychology of animal cognition: Piagetian studies. , 1987 .
[18] G. Berns,et al. Functional MRI in Awake Unrestrained Dogs , 2012, PloS one.
[19] P. Bateson,et al. Developing a framework for development: a discussion. , 2007, Developmental psychobiology.
[20] Lisa Lit,et al. Evaluating Learning Tasks Commonly Applied in Detection Dog Training , 2009 .
[21] T. Jezierski,et al. Do Trained Dogs Discriminate Individual Body Odors of Women Better than Those of Men? * , 2012, Journal of forensic sciences.
[22] Sylvain Fiset,et al. Landmark-based search memory in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). , 2007, Journal of comparative psychology.
[23] Anthony A. Wright,et al. Categorization of birds, mammals, and chimeras by pigeons , 2013, Behavioural Processes.
[24] R. J. Young,et al. The efficacy of the model–rival method when compared with operant conditioning for training domestic dogs to perform a retrieval–selection task , 2003 .
[25] How two word-trained dogs integrate pointing and naming , 2012, Animal Cognition.
[26] V. Lovic,et al. Maternal and littermate deprivation disrupts maternal behavior and social-learning of food preference in adulthood: tactile stimulation, nest odor, and social rearing prevent these effects. , 2006, Developmental psychobiology.
[27] H. Frank,et al. Comparative manipulation-test performance in ten-week-old wolves (Canis lupus) and Alaskan malamutes (Canis familiaris): A Piagetian interpretation. , 1985 .
[28] Ádám Miklósi,et al. Dog as a model for studying conspecific and heterospecific social learning , 2009 .
[29] N. Milgram,et al. Oral Administration of Adrafinil Improves Discrimination Learning in Aged Beagle Dogs , 2000, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.
[30] S. Soltysik,et al. Transfer of the "same-different' differentiation task in dogs. , 1975, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.
[31] M. Mendl,et al. Using judgement bias to measure positive affective state in dogs , 2011 .
[32] Thomas R. Zentall,et al. Imitation: definitions, evidence, and mechanisms , 2006, Animal Cognition.
[33] L. Huber,et al. Dogs imitate selectively, not necessarily rationally: reply to Kaminski et al. (2011) , 2012, Animal Behaviour.
[34] W. Roberts,et al. Can dogs (Canis familiaris) detect human deception? , 2009, Behavioural Processes.
[35] Á. Miklósi,et al. When dogs seem to lose their nose: an investigation on the use of visual and olfactory cues in communicative context between dog and owner , 2003 .
[36] R. J. Young,et al. Do domestic dogs show any evidence of being able to count? , 2002, Animal Cognition.
[37] L. Hasselbach,et al. Motivation and insight in wolf (Canis lupus) and Alaskan malamute (Canis familiaris): Visual discrimination learning , 1989 .
[38] Michael Tomasello,et al. Apes' and children's understanding of cooperative and competitive motives in a communicative situation. , 2006, Developmental science.
[39] Ludwig Huber,et al. The absence of reward induces inequity aversion in dogs , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[40] Inhibition training in wolves and dogs , 1983, Behavioural Processes.
[41] C. Cotman,et al. Visuospatial impairments in aged canines (Canis familiaris): the role of cognitive-behavioral flexibility. , 2002, Behavioral neuroscience.
[42] I. Adachi,et al. Dogs recall their owner's face upon hearing the owner's voice , 2006, Animal Cognition.
[43] C. Ades,et al. A dog at the keyboard: using arbitrary signs to communicate requests , 2008, Animal Cognition.
[44] Can dogs defy gravity? A comparison with the human infant and a non‐human primate , 2003 .
[45] Kimberly S. Kirkpatrick,et al. Tracking of the expected time to reinforcement in temporal conditioning procedures , 2003, Learning & behavior.
[46] Franz J Weissing,et al. Animal personalities: consequences for ecology and evolution. , 2012, Trends in ecology & evolution.
[47] Robert Rosenthal,et al. Can you believe my eyes? The importance of interobserver reliability statistics in observations of animal behaviour , 2009, Animal Behaviour.
[48] Ludwig Huber,et al. Obey or not obey? Dogs (Canis familiaris) behave differently in response to attentional states of their owners. , 2006, Journal of comparative psychology.
[49] Angel M. Elgier,et al. Incentive contrast in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris). , 2009, Journal of Comparative Psychology.
[50] Michael Tomasello,et al. The Domestication of Social Cognition in Dogs , 2002, Science.
[51] C. Dumas,et al. Strategy planning in dogs (Canis familiaris) in a progressive elimination task , 2006, Behavioural Processes.
[52] S. Fiset,et al. Duration of dogs' (Canis familiaris) working memory in search for disappearing objects , 2003, Animal Cognition.
[53] A. Bond,et al. Searching image in blue jays: Facilitation and interference in sequential priming , 1999 .
[54] Paul Bloom,et al. Can a Dog Learn a Word? , 2004, Science.
[55] C. Wynne,et al. Breed differences in dogs sensitivity to human points: A meta-analysis , 2009, Behavioural Processes.
[56] C. Carere,et al. Interaction between animal personality and animal cognition , 2011 .
[57] S. Shettleworth. Modularity, comparative cognition and human uniqueness , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[58] N. Spear,et al. Animal memory and learning. , 1990, Annual review of psychology.
[59] Josep Call,et al. Are there geniuses among the apes? , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[60] P. Sachdev,et al. The canine sand maze: an appetitive spatial memory paradigm sensitive to age-related change in dogs. , 2011, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[61] S. Budaev,et al. Personality Traits and Behaviour , 2011 .
[62] O. Rasa,et al. Observational learning of an acquired maternal behaviour pattern by working dog pups: an alternative training method? , 1997 .
[63] Holly C. Miller,et al. What do dogs know about hidden objects? , 2009, Behavioural Processes.
[64] C. Cotman,et al. Response latency in Canis familiaris: mental ability or mental strategy? , 2003, Behavioral neuroscience.
[65] Susan Carey,et al. Acquiring a Single New Word , 1978 .
[66] C. Wynne,et al. Wolves outperform dogs in following human social cues , 2008, Animal Behaviour.
[67] T. Howell,et al. Can dogs (canis familiaris) use a mirror to solve a problem , 2011 .
[68] Á. Miklósi,et al. Dogs respond appropriately to cues of humans’ attentional focus , 2004, Behavioural Processes.
[69] Louis D Matzel,et al. Individual Differences in the Expression of a “General” Learning Ability in Mice , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[70] John L. Fuller,et al. Genetics and the Social Behaviour of the Dog , 1965 .
[71] Jonathan J. Cooper,et al. Clever hounds: social cognition in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris) , 2003 .
[72] Domestic dogs’ (Canis familiaris) choices in reference to agreement among human informants on location of food , 2012, Animal Cognition.
[73] M. Pigliucci. Phenotypic integration: studying the ecology and evolution of complex phenotypes , 2003 .
[74] A. Thornton,et al. Individual variation in cognitive performance: developmental and evolutionary perspectives , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[75] Sylvain Fiset,et al. Evidence for averaging of distance from landmarks in the domestic dog , 2009, Behavioural Processes.
[76] Thomas R. Zentall,et al. IMITATION IN ANIMALS: EVIDENCE, FUNCTION, AND MECHANISMS , 2001, Cybern. Syst..
[77] J. Call,et al. Dogs, Canis familiaris, fail to copy intransitive actions in third-party contextual imitation tasks , 2009, Animal Behaviour.
[78] F. Schenk,et al. Olfactory and/or visual cues for spatial navigation through ontogeny: olfactory cues enable the use of visual cues. , 2003, Behavioral neuroscience.
[79] Joseph Terkel,et al. The role of context specificity in learning: the effects of training context on explosives detection in dogs , 2005, Animal Cognition.
[80] T. Caro,et al. Inter-Observer Reliability , 1979 .
[81] A. Dornhaus,et al. Psychophysics: Bees trade off foraging speed for accuracy , 2003, Nature.
[82] Ludwig Huber,et al. The role of item- and category-specific information in the discrimination of people versus nonpeople images by pigeons , 2001 .
[83] B. Osthaus,et al. Minding the gap: spatial perseveration error in dogs , 2010, Animal Cognition.
[84] Shawn E Brooks,et al. Ability of canine termite detectors to locate live termites and discriminate them from non-termite material. , 2003, Journal of economic entomology.
[85] SIMON BENHAMOU,et al. Path integration in dogs , 1998, Animal Behaviour.
[86] V. Csányi,et al. A nonverbal test of knowledge attribution: a comparative study on dogs and children , 2005, Animal Cognition.
[87] J. Fuller. Transitory effects of experiential deprivation upon reversal learning in dogs , 1966 .
[88] D. Kowalska. The method of training dogs in auditory recognition memory tasks with trial-unique stimuli. , 1997, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.
[89] D. Macdonald,et al. Variation in dog society: between resource dispersion and social flux , 1995 .
[90] P. McGreevy,et al. Development of a novel paradigm for the measurement of olfactory discrimination in dogs (Canis familiaris): A pilot study , 2012 .
[91] W. Roberts,et al. Dogs choose a human informant: Metacognition in canines , 2010, Behavioural Processes.
[92] S. Marshall-Pescini,et al. Social referencing in dog-owner dyads? , 2011, Animal Cognition.
[93] H. Adler,et al. Ontogeny of observational learning in the dog (Canis familiaris). , 1977, Developmental psychobiology.
[94] Wolfgang Heuwieser,et al. Training dogs on a scent platform for oestrus detection in cows , 2011 .
[95] S. Kundey,et al. Domesticated dogs’ (Canis familiaris) use of the solidity principle , 2010, Animal Cognition.
[96] James R. Anderson,et al. How do African grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus) perform on a delay of gratification task? , 2010, Animal Cognition.
[97] S. Shettleworth. Cognition, evolution, and behavior , 1998 .
[98] Sylvain Fiset,et al. Invisible displacement understanding in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris): the role of visual cues in search behavior , 2007, Animal Cognition.
[99] G.A.A. Schoon,et al. SCENT IDENTIFICATIONS BY DOGS (CANIS FAMILIARIS): A NEW EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN , 1997 .
[100] M. Tomasello,et al. Communication of Food Location Between Human and Dog ( Canis Familiaris ) , 1998 .
[101] Dogs, Canis familiaris, find hidden food by observing and interacting with a conspecific , 2009, Animal Behaviour.
[102] Á. Miklósi. Dog Behaviour, Evolution, and Cognition , 2015 .
[103] T. Keasar,et al. The use of numerical information by bees in foraging tasks , 2011 .
[104] C. Fabrigoule,et al. Reorganization of cues and path organisation in dogs , 1992, Behavioural Processes.
[105] M. Domjan. Pavlovian conditioning: a functional perspective. , 2005, Annual review of psychology.
[106] Michael Tomasello,et al. Domestic Dogs Use Contextual Information and Tone of Voice when following a Human Pointing Gesture , 2011, PloS one.
[107] Thomas R. Zentall,et al. Object permanence in dogs: Invisible displacement in a rotation task , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[108] E. Wilsson,et al. Are there differences between behavioral measurement methods? A comparison of the predictive validity of two ratings methods in a working dog program , 2012 .
[109] Michael Tomasello,et al. Breed Differences in Domestic Dogs' (Canis familiaris) Comprehension of Human Communicative Signals , 2009 .
[110] E. Natoli,et al. Free-ranging dogs assess the quantity of opponents in intergroup conflicts , 2010, Animal Cognition.
[111] E. Ostrander,et al. Canine Genomics and Genetics: Running with the Pack , 2005, PLoS genetics.
[112] C. Dollaghan,et al. Child meets word: "fast mapping" in preschool children. , 1985, Journal of speech and hearing research.
[113] C. Cotman,et al. Landmark discrimination learning in the dog. , 1999, Learning & memory.
[114] Á. Miklósi,et al. Dogs' Gaze Following Is Tuned to Human Communicative Signals , 2012, Current Biology.
[115] Á. Miklósi,et al. A Simple Reason for a Big Difference Wolves Do Not Look Back at Humans, but Dogs Do , 2003, Current Biology.
[116] Waiting for more: the performance of domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) on exchange tasks , 2011, Animal Cognition.
[117] Brian J Cummings,et al. Spatial learning and memory as a function of age in the dog. , 1995, Behavioral neuroscience.
[118] Michael E. Bratman,et al. Intention and Means-End Reasoning , 1981 .
[119] Á. Miklósi,et al. Why do dogs (Canis familiaris) select the empty container in an observational learning task? , 2010, Animal Cognition.
[120] N. Chapuis,et al. Short Cuts by Dogs in Natural Surroundings , 1987 .
[121] F. Paglieri,et al. The ecological rationality of delay tolerance: Insights from capuchin monkeys , 2011, Cognition.
[122] Andrew Sih,et al. Insights for Behavioral Ecology from Behavioral Syndromes. , 2008, Advances in the study of behavior.
[123] F. Doré,et al. Search behavior in cats and dogs: Interspecific differences in working memory and spatial cognition , 1996 .
[124] Robert Pasnak,et al. Object permanence in cats and dogs , 1981 .
[125] P. McConnell. Acoustic structure and receiver response in domestic dogs, Canis familiaris , 1990, Animal Behaviour.
[126] P. Shaver,et al. Pets as safe havens and secure bases: The moderating role of pet attachment orientations , 2012 .
[127] Tadeusz Jezierski,et al. INFORMATION-SEEKING BEHAVIOUR OF SNIFFER DOGS DURING MATCH-TO-SAMPLE TRAINING IN THE SCENT LINEUP , 2008 .
[128] D. Wells,et al. Prenatal olfactory learning in the domestic dog , 2006, Animal Behaviour.
[129] Pamela J. Reid,et al. Adapting to the human world: Dogs’ responsiveness to our social cues , 2009, Behavioural Processes.
[130] Thomas Suddendorf,et al. Do dogs (Canis familiaris) understand invisible displacement? , 2004, Journal of comparative psychology.
[131] B. Hare,et al. Testing the social dog hypothesis: Are dogs also more skilled than chimpanzees in non-communicative social tasks? , 2009, Behavioural Processes.
[132] S. Marshall-Pescini,et al. Are dogs (Canis familiaris) misled more by their owners than by strangers in a food choice task? , 2010, Animal Cognition.
[133] Á. Miklósi,et al. A cross-cultural comparison of reports by German Shepherd owners in Hungary and the United States of America , 2009 .
[134] V. Morell,et al. Animal behavior. Going to the dogs. , 2009, Science.
[135] M. Tomasello,et al. Simple Mechanisms Can Explain Social Learning in Domestic Dogs (Canis familiaris) , 2011 .
[136] Jonathan Gray,et al. Two-item discrimination and Hamilton search learning in infant pigtailed macaque monkeys , 2011, Behavioural Processes.
[137] Á. Miklósi,et al. Are readers of our face readers of our minds? Dogs (Canis familiaris) show situation-dependent recognition of human’s attention , 2004, Animal Cognition.
[138] M. Olson. The developmental renaissance in adaptationism. , 2012, Trends in ecology & evolution.
[139] J. Fetterman. The psychophysics of remembered duration , 1995 .
[140] Sylvain Fiset,et al. Spatial memory of domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) for hidden objects in a detour task. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[141] W. E. Bacon,et al. Reversal learning in neonatal dogs. , 1970 .
[142] K. J. Hayes,et al. Imitation in a home-raised chimpanzee. , 1952, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[143] Kenneth G. Furton,et al. Laboratory and field experiments used to identify Canis lupus var. familiaris active odor signature chemicals from drugs, explosives, and humans , 2003, Analytical and bioanalytical chemistry.
[144] Thomas T. Hills,et al. Why Aren’t We Smarter Already , 2011 .
[145] Thomas Suddendorf,et al. Mental time travel in animals? , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[146] B. L. Brown,et al. Four-pair same-different differentiation and transient memory in dogs. , 1971, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.
[147] D. Wells,et al. How many footsteps do dogs need to determine the direction of an odour trail? , 2005, Chemical senses.
[148] Alliston K. Reid,et al. Border collie comprehends object names as verbal referents , 2011, Behavioural Processes.
[149] H. Frank,et al. Evolution of canine information processing under conditions of natural and artificial selection. , 1980, Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie.
[150] S. Marshall-Pescini,et al. Dogs' Social Referencing towards Owners and Strangers , 2012, PloS one.
[151] C. Cotman,et al. Age and distraction are determinants of performance on a novel visual search task in aged Beagle dogs , 2011, AGE.
[152] S. Marshall-Pescini,et al. Do Dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) Make Counterproductive Choices Because They Are Sensitive to Human Ostensive Cues? , 2012, PloS one.
[153] A. Etienne,et al. Blindfolded dogs relocate a target through path integration , 2004, Animal Behaviour.
[154] Holly C. Miller,et al. Imitation and emulation by dogs using a bidirectional control procedure , 2009, Behavioural Processes.
[155] Joseph Terkel,et al. Formation of an Olfactory Search Image for Explosives Odours in Sniffer Dogs , 2005 .
[156] R. Baillargeon. Young infants' reasoning about the physical and spatial properties of a hidden object , 1987 .
[157] M. Mcculloch,et al. Diagnostic Accuracy of Canine Scent Detection in Early- and Late-Stage Lung and Breast Cancers , 2006, Integrative cancer therapies.
[158] M. Tomasello,et al. Domestic Dogs (Canis familiaris) Use Human and Conspecific Social Cues to Locate Hidden Food , 1999 .
[159] R Plomin,et al. Evidence for general cognitive ability (g) in heterogeneous stock mice and an analysis of potential confounds , 2002, Genes, brain, and behavior.
[160] Ádám Miklósi,et al. Interaction between individual experience and social learning in dogs , 2003, Animal Behaviour.
[161] Ádám Miklósi,et al. Comparative social cognition: From wolf and dog to humans , 2006 .
[162] J. Call,et al. The magic cup: great apes and domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) individuate objects according to their properties. , 2011, Journal of comparative psychology.
[163] Robert Plomin,et al. Genetics and general cognitive ability , 1999, Nature.
[164] J. Langlois,et al. An olfactory discrimination procedure for mice. , 2000, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[165] Angel M. Elgier,et al. Effect of reinforcement, reinforcer omission and extinction on a communicative response in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) , 2008, Behavioural Processes.
[166] Brief owner absence does not induce negative judgement bias in pet dogs , 2012, Animal Cognition.
[167] L. Huber,et al. Selective Imitation in Domestic Dogs , 2007, Current Biology.
[168] Á. Miklósi,et al. Dogs' Expectation about Signalers' Body Size by Virtue of Their Growls , 2010, PloS one.
[169] M. Tomasello,et al. Domestic dogs conceal auditory but not visual information from others , 2013, Animal Cognition.
[170] L. Huber,et al. Female but not male dogs respond to a size constancy violation , 2011, Biology Letters.
[171] F. Gaunet,et al. The response of guide dogs and pet dogs (Canis Familiaris) to cues of human referential communication (pointing and gaze) , 2009, Animal Cognition.
[172] B. Gibson,et al. Clark’s nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana) use gestures to identify the location of hidden food , 2010, Animal Cognition.
[173] Michael Tomasello,et al. Understanding of human communicative motives in domestic dogs , 2011 .
[174] D. Mills,et al. Can stimulus enhancement explain the apparent success of the model-rival technique in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris)? , 2008 .
[175] T. Stokes,et al. Discrimination and generalization. , 1992, Journal of applied behavior analysis.
[176] Jessica P. Stagner,et al. Episodic-like memory: Pigeons can report location pecked when unexpectedly asked , 2008, Behavioural Processes.
[177] W. Shepherd. Tests on Adaptive Intelligence in Dogs and Cats, as Compared with Adaptive Intelligence in Rhesus Monkeys , 1915 .
[178] Á. Miklósi,et al. Verbal attention getting as a key factor in social learning between dog (Canis familiaris) and human. , 2004, Journal of comparative psychology.
[179] C. Wynne,et al. Ontogeny's impacts on human–dog communication , 2008, Animal Behaviour.
[180] S. Dehaene,et al. Abstract representations of numbers in the animal and human brain , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.
[181] D. Mills,et al. Neophilia in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) and its implication for studies of dog cognition , 2008, Animal Cognition.
[182] D. Hovda,et al. Ontogeny of Rat Recognition Memory measured by the novel object recognition task. , 2009, Developmental psychobiology.
[183] Ádám Miklósi,et al. Dog-Human Relationship Affects Problem Solving Behavior in the Dog , 1997 .
[184] Julia Fischer,et al. A pluralistic account of word learning , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[185] Ross J. Harper,et al. Identification of dominant odor chemicals emanating from explosives for use in developing optimal training aid combinations and mimics for canine detection. , 2005, Talanta.
[186] Á. Miklósi,et al. Preference for copying unambiguous demonstrations in dogs (Canis familiaris). , 2003, Journal of comparative psychology.
[187] Márta Gácsi,et al. A friend or an enemy? Dogs' reaction to an unfamiliar person showing behavioural cues of threat and friendliness at different times , 2005 .
[188] J. Morton,et al. Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) and the radial arm maze: spatial memory and serial position effects. , 2012, Journal of comparative psychology.
[189] W. Helton. Canine Ergonomics: The Science of Working Dogs , 2009 .
[190] M. Ryan. Replication in Field Biology: The Case of the Frog-Eating Bat , 2011, Science.
[191] H. Bekkering,et al. Developmental psychology: Rational imitation in preverbal infants , 2002, Nature.
[192] S. Fiset,et al. Object permanence in domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) and gray wolves (Canis lupus). , 2013, Journal of comparative psychology.
[193] M. Hauser. Wild Minds: What Animals Really Think , 2000 .
[194] Á. Miklósi,et al. How does dominance rank status affect individual and social learning performance in the dog (Canis familiaris)? , 2007, Animal Cognition.
[195] N. Tinbergen. On aims and methods of Ethology , 2010 .
[196] B. Smuts,et al. Quantity-based judgments in the domestic dog (Canis lupus familiaris) , 2006, Animal Cognition.
[197] C. Packer,et al. Roaring and numerical assessment in contests between groups of female lions, Panthera leo , 1994, Animal Behaviour.
[198] E. Thorndike. Animal Intelligence; Experimental Studies , 2009 .
[199] S. Marshall-Pescini,et al. Social eavesdropping in the domestic dog , 2011, Animal Behaviour.
[200] Pointing following in dogs: are simple or complex cognitive mechanisms involved? , 2012, Animal Cognition.
[201] Michael Tomasello,et al. Visual perspective taking in dogs (Canis familiaris) in the presence of barriers , 2004 .
[202] Genetics of canine olfaction and receptor diversity , 2012, Mammalian Genome.
[203] Lisa Lit,et al. Effects of training paradigms on search dog performance , 2006 .
[204] M. Tomasello,et al. Making inferences about the location of hidden food: social dog, causal ape. , 2006, Journal of comparative psychology.
[205] M. Tomasello,et al. How dogs know when communication is intended for them. , 2012, Developmental science.
[206] J. Call,et al. Comparing dogs and great apes in their ability to visually track object transpositions , 2009, Animal Cognition.
[207] S. Gosling,et al. The world from a dog's point of view , 2013 .
[208] D. Pfaff,et al. Postnatal Environment Affects Behavior of Adult Transgenic Mice , 2004, Experimental biology and medicine.
[209] Florence Gaunet,et al. How do guide dogs and pet dogs (Canis familiaris) ask their owners for their toy and for playing? , 2010, Animal Cognition.
[210] C. De Lillo,et al. Association, inhibition, and object permanence in dogs' (Canis familiaris) spatial search. , 2011, Journal of comparative psychology.
[211] Linda J. Keeling,et al. The effect of time left alone at home on dog welfare , 2011 .
[212] Florence Gaunet,et al. How do guide dogs of blind owners and pet dogs of sighted owners (Canis familiaris) ask their owners for food? , 2008, Animal Cognition.
[213] S. Gosling,et al. Personality and performance in military working dogs: Reliability and predictive validity of behavioral tests , 2010 .
[214] Á. Miklósi,et al. Big thoughts in small brains? Dogs as a model for understanding human social cognition , 2007, Neuroreport.
[215] H. Frank. Wolves, Dogs, Rearing and Reinforcement: Complex Interactions Underlying Species Differences in Training and Problem-Solving Performance , 2011, Behavior genetics.
[216] D. Premack,et al. Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? , 1978, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[217] Harry Frank,et al. Comparison of problem-solving performance in six-week-old wolves and dogs , 1982, Animal Behaviour.
[218] E. Markman,et al. Word learning in dogs? , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[219] L. J. Myers,et al. The scientific foundation and efficacy of the use of canines as chemical detectors for explosives. , 2001, Talanta.
[220] Clive D. L. Wynne,et al. When do domestic dogs, Canis familiaris, start to understand human pointing? The role of ontogeny in the development of interspecies communication , 2010, Animal Behaviour.
[221] M. Tomasello,et al. Dogs, Canis familiaris, communicate with humans to request but not to inform , 2011, Animal Behaviour.
[222] Ádám Miklósi,et al. ‘The bone is mine’: affective and referential aspects of dog growls , 2010, Animal Behaviour.
[223] N. Rooney,et al. Social cognition in the domestic dog: behaviour of spectators towards participants in interspecific games , 2006, Animal Behaviour.
[224] C. Cotman,et al. Canine cognition, aging and neuropathology , 2000, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry.
[225] W. E. Bacon,et al. Discriminated instrumental learning in neonatal dogs. , 1970 .
[226] C. Wynne. Do Animals Think , 2004 .
[227] Directional tracking in the domestic dog, Canis familiaris , 2003 .
[228] L. Hiestand. A Comparison of Problem-Solving and Spatial Orientation in the Wolf (Canis lupus) and Dog (Canis familiaris) , 2011, Behavior genetics.
[229] M. Tomasello,et al. The Structure of Individual Differences in the Cognitive Abilities of Children and Chimpanzees , 2010, Psychological science.
[230] M. Nagasawa,et al. Dogs can discriminate human smiling faces from blank expressions , 2011, Animal Cognition.
[231] Do dogs distinguish rational from irrational acts? , 2011, Animal Behaviour.
[232] Gabriella Lakatos,et al. Comprehension and utilisation of pointing gestures and gazing in dog–human communication in relatively complex situations , 2011, Animal Cognition.
[233] C. Cotman,et al. Concept abstraction in the aging dog: development of a protocol using successive discrimination and size concept tasks , 2004, Behavioural Brain Research.
[234] Bernhard Voelkl,et al. The evolution of imitation: what do the capacities of non-human animals tell us about the mechanisms of imitation? , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[235] C. Ades,et al. Two-Item Sentence Comprehension by a Dog (Canis familiaris) , 2012, PloS one.
[236] Christina M. Brown,et al. Friends with benefits: on the positive consequences of pet ownership. , 2011, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[237] Alison M Bell,et al. An evolutionary ecology of individual differences. , 2012, Ecology letters.
[238] T. Howell,et al. Puppy power! Using social cognition research tasks to improve socialization practices for domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) , 2011 .
[239] C. Dumas. Figurative and spatial information and search behavior in dogs (Canis familiaris) , 1998, Behavioural Processes.
[240] Daniel Mills,et al. Word Generalization by a Dog (Canis familiaris): Is Shape Important? , 2012, PloS one.
[241] Á. Miklósi,et al. Dogs (Canis familiaris) learn from their owners via observation in a manipulation task. , 2003, Journal of comparative psychology.
[242] Josep Call,et al. What do bonobos (Pan paniscus) understand about physical contact? , 2006, Journal of comparative psychology.
[243] C. Heyes,et al. Automatic imitation in dogs , 2011, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[244] J. W. Rudy,et al. Ontogeny of spatial navigation behaviors in the rat: dissociation of "proximal"- and "distal"-cue-based behaviors. , 1987, Behavioral neuroscience.
[245] Michael Tomasello,et al. Human-like social skills in dogs? , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[246] R. Byrne. Emulation in apes: verdict ‘not proven’ , 2002 .
[247] F. Doré,et al. Search behavior in various breeds of adult dogs (Canis familiaris): object permanence and olfactory cues. , 1992, Journal of comparative psychology.
[248] Paola Valsecchi,et al. Does training make you smarter? The effects of training on dogs’ performance (Canis familiaris) in a problem solving task , 2008, Behavioural Processes.
[249] William S. Helton,et al. Physical size matters in the domestic dog's (Canis lupus familiaris) ability to use human pointing cues , 2010, Behavioural Processes.
[250] A. Mustaca,et al. Do more sociable dogs gaze longer to the human face than less sociable ones? , 2012, Behavioural Processes.
[251] S. Gosling,et al. Temperament and personality in dogs (Canis familiaris): A review and evaluation of past research , 2005 .
[252] Robert Pasnak,et al. Assessment of Stage 6 object permanence , 1988 .
[253] Márta Gácsi,et al. The effect of development and individual differences in pointing comprehension of dogs , 2009, Animal Cognition.
[254] Brian Hare,et al. From Nonhuman to Human Mind , 2007 .
[255] M. Corballis,et al. Behavioural evidence for mental time travel in nonhuman animals , 2010, Behavioural Brain Research.
[256] J. Millot. Olfactory and visual cues in the interaction systems between dogs and children , 1994, Behavioural Processes.
[257] L. Kruglyak,et al. Genetic Structure of the Purebred Domestic Dog , 2004, Science.
[258] Kristina F. Pattison,et al. The case of the disappearing bone: Dogs’ understanding of the physical properties of objects , 2010, Behavioural Processes.
[259] C. Thinus-Blanc,et al. Dissociation of Mechanisms Involved in Dogs’ Oriented Displacements , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.
[260] Sylvain Fiset,et al. Egocentric search for disappearing objects in domestic dogs: evidence for a geometric hypothesis of direction , 2005, Animal Cognition.
[261] Á. Miklósi,et al. Explaining Dog Wolf Differences in Utilizing Human Pointing Gestures: Selection for Synergistic Shifts in the Development of Some Social Skills , 2009, PloS one.
[262] C. Wynne,et al. Can your dog read your mind? Understanding the causes of canine perspective taking , 2011, Learning & behavior.
[263] L. Huber,et al. Inferential reasoning by exclusion in pigeons, dogs, and humans , 2008, Animal Cognition.
[264] C. Wynne,et al. Ontogeny and phylogeny: both are essential to human-sensitive behaviour in the genus Canis , 2010, Animal Behaviour.
[265] K. Laland,et al. Social Learning in Animals: Empirical Studies and Theoretical Models , 2005 .
[266] Michael McCulloch,et al. Canine scent detection of human cancers: A review of methods and accuracy , 2010 .
[267] HighWire Press. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London , 1781, The London Medical Journal.
[268] M. Tomasello,et al. Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use a physical marker to locate hidden food , 2005, Animal Cognition.
[269] K. Fujita,et al. Incidental memory in dogs (Canis familiaris): adaptive behavioral solution at an unexpected memory test , 2012, Animal Cognition.
[270] C Beaulieu,et al. Spatial encoding of hidden objects in dogs (Canis familiaris). , 2000, Journal of comparative psychology.
[271] W. E. Bacon,et al. Avoidance learning in neonatal dogs. , 1970, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[272] M. Laska,et al. A two-choice discrimination method to assess olfactory performance in pigtailed macaques, Macaca nemestrina , 2001, Physiology & Behavior.
[273] Á. Miklósi,et al. When rank counts — dominant dogs learn better from a human demonstrator in a two-action test , 2012 .
[274] J. Call,et al. Word Learning in a Domestic Dog: Evidence for "Fast Mapping" , 2004, Science.
[275] N. Milgram,et al. The canine as a model of human cognitive aging: Recent developments , 2000, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry.
[276] Á. Miklósi,et al. Differential Sensitivity to Human Communication in Dogs, Wolves, and Human Infants , 2009, Science.
[277] M. Tomasello,et al. Dogs (Canis familiaris) Evaluate Humans on the Basis of Direct Experiences Only , 2012, PloS one.
[278] M. Tomasello,et al. Cues to food location that domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) of different ages do and do not use , 2000, Animal Cognition.
[279] M. Tomasello,et al. Domestic dogs comprehend human communication with iconic signs. , 2009, Developmental science.
[280] L. Huber,et al. Visual categorization of natural stimuli by domestic dogs , 2008, Animal Cognition.
[281] R. Conduit,et al. Auditory stimulus discrimination recorded in dogs, as indicated by mismatch negativity (MMN) , 2012, Behavioural Processes.
[282] N. Milgram,et al. Extensive spatial training does not negate age differences in response latency , 2006, Brain Research.
[283] Ludwig Huber,et al. The effect of ostensive cues on dogs’ performance in a manipulative social learning task , 2009 .
[284] C. Wynne,et al. Reevaluating canine perspective-taking behavior , 2011, Learning & behavior.
[285] C. Locurto,et al. Individual differences and a spatial learning factor in two strains of mice (Mus musculus). , 1998 .
[286] John C. Sagebiel,et al. Olfaction-based Detection Distance: A Quantitative Analysis of How Far Away Dogs Recognize Tortoise Odor and Follow It to Source , 2008, Sensors.
[287] Thomas D. Sambrook,et al. Use of human-given cues by domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) and horses (Equus caballus) , 2000, Animal Cognition.
[288] Britt Anderson,et al. Evidence from the rat for a general factor that underlies cognitive performance and that relates to brain size: intelligence? , 1993, Neuroscience Letters.
[289] Á. Miklósi,et al. Friend or foe: Context dependent sensitivity to human behaviour in dogs , 2010 .
[290] D. Mills,et al. Behavioural and physiological correlates of impulsivity in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris) , 2012, Physiology & Behavior.
[291] Á. Miklósi,et al. A comparative analysis of animals' understanding of the human pointing gesture , 2006, Animal Cognition.
[292] C. Wynne,et al. What did domestication do to dogs? A new account of dogs' sensitivity to human actions , 2010, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.
[293] Robert Plomin,et al. The genetics of G in human and mouse , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[294] L. de Sousa,et al. Auditory contagious yawning in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris): first evidence for social modulation , 2012, Animal Cognition.
[295] Julie B. Schweitzer,et al. Handler beliefs affect scent detection dog outcomes , 2011, Animal Cognition.
[296] C. Gallistel,et al. Non-verbal numerical cognition: from reals to integers , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[297] Á. Miklósi,et al. On the hunt for the gene of perspective taking: pitfalls in methodology , 2011, Learning & behavior.
[298] Gordon M. Burghardt,et al. Perspectives – Minimizing Observer Bias in Behavioral Studies: A Review and Recommendations , 2012 .
[299] Á. Miklósi,et al. Comparative social cognition: what can dogs teach us? , 2004, Animal Behaviour.
[300] Ádám Miklósi,et al. Dogs' (Canis familiaris) responsiveness to human pointing gestures. , 2002 .
[301] A. Reyes,et al. Domesticated dogs (Canis familiaris) react to what others can and cannot hear , 2010 .
[302] Á. Miklósi,et al. The pet dogs ability for learning from a human demonstrator in a detour task is independent from the breed and age , 2005 .
[303] Linda B. Smith,et al. The importance of shape in early lexical learning , 1988 .
[304] W. Heron,et al. The effects of restricting early experience on the problem-solving capacity of dogs. , 1954, Canadian journal of psychology.
[305] M. West-Eberhard. Developmental plasticity and evolution , 2003 .
[306] Can domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use referential emotional expressions to locate hidden food? , 2012, Animal Cognition.
[307] C. Wynne,et al. A history of dogs as subjects in North American experimental psychological research , 2011 .
[308] Á. Miklósi,et al. Dog-logic: inferential reasoning in a two-way choice task and its restricted use , 2007, Animal Behaviour.
[309] Norton W. Milgram,et al. Cognitive Experience and Its Effect on Age-Dependent Cognitive Decline in Beagle Dogs , 2003, Neurochemical Research.
[310] V. Csányi,et al. Intentional behaviour in dog-human communication: an experimental analysis of “showing” behaviour in the dog , 2000, Animal Cognition.
[311] J. Call,et al. Prospective object search in dogs: mixed evidence for knowledge of What and Where , 2007, Animal Cognition.
[312] Á. Miklósi,et al. The role of visual cues in the comprehension of the human pointing signals in dogs. , 2007 .
[313] Alexandra Horowitz. Inside of a Dog: What Dogs See, Smell, and Know , 2009 .
[314] Á. Miklósi,et al. Species-specific differences and similarities in the behavior of hand-raised dog and wolf pups in social situations with humans. , 2005, Developmental psychobiology.
[315] S. O'hara,et al. A test of the yawning contagion and emotional connectedness hypothesis in dogs, Canis familiaris , 2011, Animal Behaviour.
[316] P. Seymour,et al. Dose-specific effects of scopolamine on canine cognition: Impairment of visuospatial memory, but not visuospatial discrimination , 2004, Psychopharmacology.
[317] C. Cotman,et al. Size and reversal learning in the beagle dog as a measure of executive function and inhibitory control in aging. , 2003, Learning & memory.
[318] F. Range,et al. Dogs are able to solve a means-end task , 2011, Animal Cognition.
[319] D. Mills,et al. More than just a word: non-semantic command variables affect obedience in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris) , 2005 .
[320] C. Cotman,et al. A comparison of egocentric and allocentric age-dependent spatial learning in the beagle dog , 2005, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry.
[321] Á. Miklósi,et al. Reproducing human actions and action sequences: “Do as I Do!” in a dog , 2006, Animal Cognition.
[322] Sylvain Gagnon,et al. Search behavior of dogs (Canis familiaris) in invisible displacement problems , 1993 .
[323] D. Kimbrough Oller,et al. Vocabulary Learning in a Yorkshire Terrier: Slow Mapping of Spoken Words , 2012, PloS one.
[324] J. G. Burns. Impulsive bees forage better: the advantage of quick, sometimes inaccurate foraging decisions , 2005, Animal Behaviour.
[325] Dare A. Baldwin,et al. Early referential understanding: Infants' ability to recognize referential acts for what they are. , 1993 .
[326] R. Pereira,et al. Ability of Bed Bug-Detecting Canines to Locate Live Bed Bugs and Viable Bed Bug Eggs , 2008, Journal of economic entomology.
[327] Alexandra Horowitz. Theory of mind in dogs? Examining method and concept , 2011, Learning & behavior.
[328] A. Mustaca,et al. Communication between domestic dogs and humans: effects of shelter housing upon the gaze to the human , 2011, Animal Cognition.
[329] Thomas R Zentall,et al. Imitation and affordance learning by pigeons (Columba livia). , 2003, Journal of comparative psychology.
[330] M. Tomasello,et al. The early ontogeny of human–dog communication , 2008, Animal Behaviour.
[331] C. A. Kiddy,et al. Estrus-related odors in milk detected by trained dogs. , 1984, Journal of dairy science.
[332] Torben Dabelsteen,et al. The effect of frequency and duration of training sessions on acquisition and long-term memory in dogs , 2011 .
[333] K. Phillips,et al. Do dogs (Canis familiaris) show contagious yawning? , 2009, Animal Cognition.
[334] Michael Tomasello,et al. Domestic dogs are sensitive to a human's perspective , 2009 .
[335] N. Milgram,et al. Cognitive functions and aging in the dog: acquisition of nonspatial visual tasks , 1994 .
[336] Á. Miklósi,et al. Modelling behavioural evolution and cognition in canines: Some problematic issues , 2012 .
[337] Á. Miklósi,et al. Comprehension of human communicative signs in pet dogs (Canis familiaris). , 2001, Journal of comparative psychology.
[338] C. Willis,et al. Olfactory detection of human bladder cancer by dogs: proof of principle study , 2004, BMJ : British Medical Journal.
[339] N. Milgram,et al. Effects of age on measures of complex working memory span in the beagle dog (Canis familiaris) using two versions of a spatial list learning paradigm. , 2003, Learning & memory.
[340] T. Bugnyar,et al. Social attention in keas, dogs, and human children , 2008, Animal Cognition.
[341] A. Senju,et al. Dogs catch human yawns , 2008, Biology Letters.
[342] Samuel D. Gosling,et al. Personality Consistency in Dogs: A Meta-Analysis , 2013, PloS one.
[343] Melinda A. Novak,et al. Can memory requirements account for species' differences in invisible displacement tasks? , 1999 .
[344] M. Fukuzawa,et al. The effect of human command phonetic characteristics on auditory cognition in dogs (Canis familiaris). , 2005, Journal of comparative psychology.
[345] Angel M. Elgier,et al. Author's Personal Copy Behavioural Processes , 2022 .
[346] A. Dickinson,et al. Scrub jays (Aphelocoma coerulescens) remember the relative time of caching as well as the location and content of their caches. , 1999, Journal of comparative psychology.
[347] S. Kundey,et al. Reputation-like inference in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) , 2011, Animal Cognition.
[348] Angel M. Elgier,et al. Communication between domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) and humans: Dogs are good learners , 2009, Behavioural Processes.
[349] F. Gaunet,et al. Functionally referential and intentional communication in the domestic dog: effects of spatial and social contexts , 2011, Animal Cognition.
[350] N. Milgram,et al. An investigation of the relationship between response latency across several cognitive tasks in the beagle dog , 2005, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry.
[351] Sue Taylor Parker,et al. Object manipulation, tool use and sensorimotor intelligence as feeding adaptations in cebus monkeys and great apes , 1977 .
[352] Cynthia Schuck-Paim,et al. Means to an end: Neotropical parrots manage to pull strings to meet their goals , 2009, Animal Cognition.
[353] Á. Miklósi,et al. Dogs can discriminate barks from different situations , 2008 .
[354] Á. Miklósi,et al. Does the A-not-B error in adult pet dogs indicate sensitivity to human communication? , 2012, Animal Cognition.
[355] S. Marshall-Pescini,et al. Comment on “Differential Sensitivity to Human Communication in Dogs, Wolves, and Human Infants” , 2010, Science.
[356] C. Cotman,et al. Use of a delayed non-matching to position task to model age-dependent cognitive decline in the dog , 2000, Behavioural Brain Research.
[357] W. Roberts,et al. Spatial memory in dogs (Canis familiaris) on a radial maze. , 2010, Journal of comparative psychology.
[358] C. Wynne,et al. Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use human gestures but not nonhuman tokens to find hidden food. , 2008, Journal of comparative psychology.
[359] D. Hebb,et al. Individual differences in dogs: preliminary report on the effects of early experience. , 1951, Canadian journal of psychology.
[360] M. Tomasello,et al. Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) are sensitive to the attentional state of humans. , 2003, Journal of comparative psychology.
[361] Norton W. Milgram,et al. Landmark discrimination learning in the dog: effects of age, an antioxidant fortified food, and cognitive strategy , 2002, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[362] Márta Gácsi,et al. Distinguishing logic from association in the solution of an invisible displacement task by children (Homo sapiens) and dogs (Canis familiaris): using negation of disjunction. , 2001 .
[363] Sylvain Gagnon,et al. Cross-sectional study of object permanence in domestic puppies (Canis familiaris). , 1994 .
[364] Angel M. Elgier,et al. Learning and owner–stranger effects on interspecific communication in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) , 2009, Behavioural Processes.
[365] A. Weiss,et al. Traits and genotypes may predict the successful training of drug detection dogs , 2007 .
[366] F. Range,et al. The Influence of the Relationship and Motivation on Inequity Aversion in Dogs , 2012 .
[367] S. Floresco,et al. Acute Stress Induces Selective Alterations in Cost/Benefit Decision-Making , 2012, Neuropsychopharmacology.
[368] James M. Johnston,et al. Training and maintaining the performance of dogs (Canis familiaris) on an increasing number of odor discriminations in a controlled setting , 2002 .
[369] M. Tomasello,et al. Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) coordinate their actions in a problem-solving task , 2013, Animal Cognition.