Two distinct neural mechanisms underlying indirect reciprocity

Significance Humans help strangers even if the strangers will not directly help them in the future. The so-called indirect reciprocity seems to support large-scale cooperation in human society. We revealed functional and anatomical neural bases of two types of indirect reciprocity by combining group and neuroimaging experiments. Reputation-based indirect reciprocity activated the precuneus, a brain region associated with self-centered cognition. Indirect reciprocity occurring as a succession of pay-it-forward behaviors specifically recruited the anterior insula, a region related to affective empathy. Furthermore, task-irrelevant neural fingerprints of these brain regions are predictive of the individual’s tendency of cooperation. These results in particular explain why we often conduct seemingly irrational cooperation such as pay-it-forward reciprocity. Cooperation is a hallmark of human society. Humans often cooperate with strangers even if they will not meet each other again. This so-called indirect reciprocity enables large-scale cooperation among nonkin and can occur based on a reputation mechanism or as a succession of pay-it-forward behavior. Here, we provide the functional and anatomical neural evidence for two distinct mechanisms governing the two types of indirect reciprocity. Cooperation occurring as reputation-based reciprocity specifically recruited the precuneus, a region associated with self-centered cognition. During such cooperative behavior, the precuneus was functionally connected with the caudate, a region linking rewards to behavior. Furthermore, the precuneus of a cooperative subject had a strong resting-state functional connectivity (rsFC) with the caudate and a large gray matter volume. In contrast, pay-it-forward reciprocity recruited the anterior insula (AI), a brain region associated with affective empathy. The AI was functionally connected with the caudate during cooperation occurring as pay-it-forward reciprocity, and its gray matter volume and rsFC with the caudate predicted the tendency of such cooperation. The revealed difference is consistent with the existing results of evolutionary game theory: although reputation-based indirect reciprocity robustly evolves as a self-interested behavior in theory, pay-it-forward indirect reciprocity does not on its own. The present study provides neural mechanisms underlying indirect reciprocity and suggests that pay-it-forward reciprocity may not occur as myopic profit maximization but elicit emotional rewards.

[1]  S. Asch Opinions and Social Pressure , 1955, Nature.

[2]  W. Hamilton,et al.  The Evolution of Cooperation , 1984 .

[3]  Karen S. Cook,et al.  Generalized Exchange and Social Dilemmas , 1993 .

[4]  J. Schotland,et al.  Generalized reciprocity. , 1998, Journal of magnetic resonance.

[5]  M. Nowak,et al.  Evolution of indirect reciprocity by image scoring , 1998, Nature.

[6]  Martin Dufwenberg,et al.  An experimental test of direct and indirect reciprocity in case of complete and incomplete information , 2000 .

[7]  O. Leimar,et al.  Evolution of cooperation through indirect reciprocity , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[8]  M. Dufwenberg,et al.  Direct versus indirect reciprocity : An experiment , 2001 .

[9]  Daniel Houser,et al.  A functional imaging study of cooperation in two-person reciprocal exchange , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[10]  M. Milinski,et al.  Cooperation through indirect reciprocity: image scoring or standing strategy? , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[11]  G. Pagnoni,et al.  A Neural Basis for Social Cooperation , 2002, Neuron.

[12]  U. Fischbacher,et al.  The nature of human altruism , 2003, Nature.

[13]  Karl Sigmund,et al.  The logic of reprobation: assessment and action rules for indirect reciprocation. , 2004, Journal of theoretical biology.

[14]  Yoh Iwasa,et al.  How should we define goodness?--reputation dynamics in indirect reciprocity. , 2004, Journal of theoretical biology.

[15]  P. Richerson,et al.  Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. , 2004 .

[16]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Reward representations and reward-related learning in the human brain: insights from neuroimaging , 2004, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[17]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Empathy for Pain Involves the Affective but not Sensory Components of Pain , 2004, Science.

[18]  Caroline F. Zink,et al.  Neurobiological Correlates of Social Conformity and Independence During Mental Rotation , 2005, Biological Psychiatry.

[19]  S. Quartz,et al.  Getting to Know You: Reputation and Trust in a Two-Person Economic Exchange , 2005, Science.

[20]  M. Delgado,et al.  Perceptions of moral character modulate the neural systems of reward during the trust game , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  M. Nowak,et al.  Evolution of indirect reciprocity , 2005, Nature.

[22]  Jeffrey C. Cooper,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging of reward prediction , 2005, Current opinion in neurology.

[23]  M. Taborsky,et al.  Contingent movement and cooperation evolve under generalized reciprocity , 2005, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[24]  A. Cavanna,et al.  The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[25]  A. Schram,et al.  Social status and group norms: Indirect reciprocity in a repeated helping experiment , 2006 .

[26]  Sébastien Roch,et al.  Upstream reciprocity and the evolution of gratitude , 2007, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[27]  D. DeSteno,et al.  Gratitude and Prosocial Behavior , 2006, Psychological science.

[28]  Walter Schneider,et al.  The cognitive control network: Integrated cortical regions with dissociable functions , 2007, NeuroImage.

[29]  Andreas Kleinschmidt,et al.  A structural neural deficit in adolescents with conduct disorder and its association with lack of empathy , 2007, NeuroImage.

[30]  David R. Schaefer,et al.  Building Solidarity through Generalized Exchange: A Theory of Reciprocity1 , 2007, American Journal of Sociology.

[31]  J. Andreoni Social Image and the 50-50 Norm: A Theoretical and Experimental Analysis of Audience Effects , 2009 .

[32]  J. Mattausch,et al.  Cooperation among strangers with limited information about reputation , 2008 .

[33]  G. Pagnoni,et al.  The neural correlates of the affective response to unreciprocated cooperation , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  Daniel J. Rankin,et al.  Assortment and the Evolution of Generalized Reciprocity , 2009, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.

[35]  M. Iacoboni Imitation, empathy, and mirror neurons. , 2009, Annual review of psychology.

[36]  Frank Van Overwalle,et al.  Understanding others' actions and goals by mirror and mentalizing systems: A meta-analysis , 2009, NeuroImage.

[37]  G. Fernández,et al.  Reinforcement Learning Signal Predicts Social Conformity , 2009, Neuron.

[38]  Dirk Engelmann,et al.  Indirect Reciprocity and Strategic Reputation Building in an Experimental Helping Game , 2002, Games Econ. Behav..

[39]  A. Craig,et al.  How do you feel — now? The anterior insula and human awareness , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[40]  Uta Frith,et al.  Reputation management in the age of the world-wide web , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[41]  C. Sripada,et al.  Reputation for reciprocity engages the brain reward center , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[42]  Naoki Masuda,et al.  Upstream reciprocity in heterogeneous networks. , 2009, Journal of theoretical biology.

[43]  Nicholas A. Christakis,et al.  Cooperative behavior cascades in human social networks , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[44]  Norihiro Sadato,et al.  Processing of the Incentive for Social Approval in the Ventral Striatum during Charitable Donation , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[45]  Claus Lamm,et al.  Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain , 2011, NeuroImage.

[46]  Colin F Camerer,et al.  Insensitivity to social reputation in autism , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[47]  R. Mar The neural bases of social cognition and story comprehension. , 2011, Annual review of psychology.

[48]  G. Fernández,et al.  Downregulation of the Posterior Medial Frontal Cortex Prevents Social Conformity , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  K. Ochsner,et al.  The neuroscience of empathy: progress, pitfalls and promise , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.

[50]  Geraint Rees,et al.  Inter-individual differences in empathy are reflected in human brain structure , 2012, NeuroImage.

[51]  T. Singer,et al.  The neural basis of empathy. , 2012, Annual review of neuroscience.

[52]  Keise Izuma,et al.  The neural basis of social influence and attitude change , 2013, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[53]  David G. Rand,et al.  Human cooperation , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.