Update on "What" and "Where" in Spatial Language: A New Division of Labor for Spatial Terms.

In this article, I revisit Landau and Jackendoff's () paper, "What and where in spatial language and spatial cognition," proposing a friendly amendment and reformulation. The original paper emphasized the distinct geometries that are engaged when objects are represented as members of object kinds (named by count nouns), versus when they are represented as figure and ground in spatial expressions (i.e., play the role of arguments of spatial prepositions). We provided empirical and theoretical arguments for the link between these distinct representations in spatial language and their accompanying nonlinguistic neural representations, emphasizing the "what" and "where" systems of the visual system. In the present paper, I propose a second division of labor between two classes of spatial prepositions in English that appear to be quite distinct. One class includes prepositions such as in and on, whose core meanings engage force-dynamic, functional relationships between objects, with geometry only a marginal player. The second class includes prepositions such as above/below and right/left, whose core meanings engage geometry, with force-dynamic relationships a passing or irrelevant variable. The insight that objects' force-dynamic relationships matter to spatial terms' uses is not new; but thinking of these terms as a distinct set within spatial language has theoretical and empirical consequences that are new. I propose three such consequences, rooted in the fact that geometric knowledge is highly constrained and early-emerging in life, while force-dynamic knowledge of objects and their interactions is relatively unconstrained and needs to be learned piecemeal over a lengthy timeline. First, the two classes will engage different learning problems, with different developmental trajectories for both first and second language learners; second, the classes will naturally lead to different degrees of cross-linguistic variation; and third, they may be rooted in different neural representations.

[1]  Eve V. Clark,et al.  Non-linguistic strategies and the acquisition of word meanings , 1973 .

[2]  Clark C. Presson,et al.  Mental rotation and the perspective problem , 1973 .

[3]  H. H. Clark SPACE, TIME, SEMANTICS, AND THE CHILD , 1973 .

[4]  E. Clark,et al.  Strategies in the acquisition of deixis , 1978, Journal of Child Language.

[5]  D. Slobin,et al.  The development of locative expressions in English, Italian, Serbo-Croatian and Turkish , 1977, Journal of Child Language.

[6]  R. Passingham The hippocampus as a cognitive map J. O'Keefe & L. Nadel, Oxford University Press, Oxford (1978). 570 pp., £25.00 , 1979, Neuroscience.

[7]  Leslie G. Ungerleider Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .

[8]  Leonard Talmy,et al.  How Language Structures Space , 1983 .

[9]  E. Spelke,et al.  Spatial knowledge in a young blind child , 1984, Cognition.

[10]  D. Levine,et al.  Two visual systems in mental imagery , 1985, Neurology.

[11]  Barbara Landau,et al.  Early map use as an unlearned ability , 1986, Cognition.

[12]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  The importance of shape in early lexical learning , 1988 .

[13]  R U Muller,et al.  Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. I. Description and quantitative analysis , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[14]  Melissa Bowerman,et al.  Topological relations picture series , 1992 .

[15]  B. Landau,et al.  “What” and “where” in spatial language and spatial cognition , 1993 .

[16]  J. Huttenlocher,et al.  The Coding of Spatial Location in Young Children , 1994, Cognitive Psychology.

[17]  P. Brown The INs and ONs of Tzeltal locative expressions: the semantics of static descriptions of location , 1994 .

[18]  M. Tarr,et al.  Spatial language and spatial representation , 1995, Cognition.

[19]  P. Quinn,et al.  Development of Categorical Representations for "Above" and "Below" Spatial Relations in 3- to 7-Month-Old Infants. , 1996 .

[20]  E. Spelke,et al.  Modularity and development: the case of spatial reorientation , 1996, Cognition.

[21]  D. Levine,et al.  Two visual systems in mental imagery: dissociation of 'what' and 'where' in imagery disorders due to bilateral posterior cerebral lesions , 1996 .

[22]  J. Mehler,et al.  LANGUAGE AND COGNITION , 1998 .

[23]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Object Shape, Object Function, and Object Name , 1998 .

[24]  David P. Wilkins,et al.  Semantic typology and spatial conceptualization , 1998 .

[25]  Silvia P. Gennari,et al.  Knowing versus Naming: Similarity and the Linguistic Categorization of Artifacts , 1999 .

[26]  S. Garrod,et al.  In and on: investigating the functional geometry of spatial prepositions , 1999, Cognition.

[27]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Topographical disorientation: a synthesis and taxonomy. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[28]  Laura A. Carlson-Radvansky,et al.  “What” Effects on “Where”: Functional Influences on Spatial Relations , 1999 .

[29]  Nora S. Newcombe,et al.  Infants' coding of location in continuous space. , 1999 .

[30]  J. O’Keefe,et al.  Modeling place fields in terms of the cortical inputs to the hippocampus , 2000, Hippocampus.

[31]  J O'Keefe,et al.  Vector grammar, places, and the functional role of the spatial prepositions in English , 2001 .

[32]  A. Damasio,et al.  Neural Correlates of Naming Actions and of Naming Spatial Relations , 2001, NeuroImage.

[33]  J. Huttenlocher,et al.  Toddlers' use of metric information and landmarks to reorient. , 2001, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[34]  Z Kourtzi,et al.  Representation of Perceived Object Shape by the Human Lateral Occipital Complex , 2001, Science.

[35]  Kenny R. Coventry,et al.  The interplay between geometry and function in the comprehension of''over , 2001 .

[36]  Laura A. Carlson,et al.  Grounding spatial language in perception: an empirical and computational investigation. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[37]  L. Gleitman,et al.  Turning the tables: language and spatial reasoning , 2002, Cognition.

[38]  L. Buxbaum,et al.  Knowledge of object manipulation and object function: dissociations in apraxic and nonapraxic subjects , 2002, Brain and Language.

[39]  Dedre Gentner,et al.  Factors Involved in the Use of In and On , 2003 .

[40]  S. Levinson,et al.  'Natural Concepts' in the Spatial Topologial Domain--Adpositional Meanings in Crosslinguistic Perspective: An Exercise in Semantic Typology , 2003 .

[41]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Two different streams form the dorsal visual system: anatomy and functions , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[42]  D. Tranel,et al.  A Double Dissociation Between the Meanings of Action Verbs and Locative Prepositions , 2003, Neurocase.

[43]  D. Tranel,et al.  Neuroanatomical correlates of locative prepositions , 2004, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[44]  Paul C Quinn,et al.  Spatial representation by young infants: Categorization of spatial relations or sensitivity to a crossing primitive? , 2004, Memory & cognition.

[45]  Kenny R. Coventry,et al.  Seeing, saying and acting: The psychological semantics of spatial prepositions , 2004 .

[46]  N. Newcombe,et al.  Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? squaring theory and evidence , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[47]  Barbara Landau,et al.  Parallels between spatial cognition and spatial language: Evidence from Williams syndrome , 2005 .

[48]  Christian J. Rapold,et al.  Cognitive cladistics and cultural override in Hominid spatial cognition , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[49]  Penelope Brown,et al.  A sketch of the grammar of space in Tzeltal , 2006 .

[50]  H. Diessel Demonstratives, joint attention, and the emergence of grammar , 2006 .

[51]  Laura Lakusta,et al.  Spatial language and spatial representation , 2006 .

[52]  Stephen C. Levinson,et al.  Grammars of space: Explorations in cognitive diversity , 2006 .

[53]  William Lopez,et al.  Attention Unites Form and Function in Spatial Language , 2006, Spatial Cogn. Comput..

[54]  S. Kastner,et al.  Two hierarchically organized neural systems for object information in human visual cortex , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[55]  Marianella Casasola,et al.  The Development of Infants' Spatial Categories , 2008 .

[56]  Barbara Landau,et al.  More Than Meets the Eye , 2008, Psychological science.

[57]  T. Talavage,et al.  Neuroanatomical distribution of five semantic components of verbs: Evidence from fMRI , 2008, Brain and Language.

[58]  S. Neggers,et al.  Neural correlates of locative prepositions , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[59]  Denise H. Wu,et al.  Neural substrates of processing path and manner information of a moving event , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[60]  Michele I. Feist Space Between Languages , 2008, Cogn. Sci..

[61]  Barbara Landau,et al.  Spatial representation across species: geometry, language, and maps , 2009, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[62]  Dedre Gentner,et al.  Why some spatial semantic categories are harder to learn than others: The typological prevalence hypothesis , 2009 .

[63]  Anjan Chatterjee,et al.  The Neural Basis for Spatial Relations , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[64]  Thomas J. Wills,et al.  Development of the Hippocampal Cognitive Map in Preweanling Rats , 2010, Science.

[65]  Barbara Landau,et al.  Developmental Decline in the Acquisition of Spatial Language , 2010 .

[66]  Daniel C. Richardson,et al.  Spatial language, visual attention, and perceptual simulation , 2010, Brain and Language.

[67]  C. Connor,et al.  Neural representations for object perception: structure, category, and adaptive coding. , 2011, Annual review of neuroscience.

[68]  Marijn E. Struiksma,et al.  Spatial Language Processing in the Blind: Evidence for a Supramodal Representation and Cortical Reorganization , 2011, PloS one.

[69]  Shannon E. McCarthy,et al.  Kindergarten children's sensitivity to geometry in maps. , 2011, Developmental science.

[70]  Anna Papafragou,et al.  Spatial reasoning in Tenejapan Mayans , 2011, Cognition.

[71]  Daniel Y. Kimberg,et al.  Language, perception, and the schematic representation of spatial relations , 2012, Brain and Language.

[72]  Sang Ah Lee,et al.  Navigation as a source of geometric knowledge: Young children’s use of length, angle, distance, and direction in a reorientation task , 2012, Cognition.

[73]  E. Bialystok,et al.  Perspective-Taking Ability in Bilingual Children: Extending Advantages in Executive Control to Spatial Reasoning. , 2013, Cognitive development.

[74]  T. Bonhoeffer,et al.  Grid cells and cortical representation , 2014, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[75]  Roberta L. Klatzky,et al.  Ventral and Dorsal Visual Stream Contributions to the Perception of Object Shape and Object Location , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[76]  M. Miozzo,et al.  How verbs and non-verbal categories navigate the syntax/semantics interface: Insights from cognitive neuropsychology , 2014, Cognition.

[77]  Dedre Gentner,et al.  Well-Hidden Regularities: Abstract Uses of in and on Retain an Aspect of Their Spatial Meaning , 2015, Cogn. Sci..

[78]  Barbara Landau,et al.  Similarity and Variation in the Distribution of Spatial Expressions Across Three Languages , 2015, CogSci.

[79]  Barbara Landau,et al.  Systematic feature variation underlies adults' and children's use of in and on , 2016, CogSci.

[80]  Anna Shusterman,et al.  Frames of reference in spatial language acquisition , 2016, Cognitive Psychology.

[81]  Barbara Landau,et al.  The importance of lexical verbs in the acquisition of spatial prepositions: The case of in and on , 2016, Cognition.

[82]  Barbara Landau,et al.  Containment and Support: Core and Complexity in Spatial Language Learning. , 2017, Cognitive science.