Infants' early visual attention and social engagement as developmental precursors to joint attention.

This study examined infants' early visual attention (at 1 month of age) and social engagement (4 months) as predictors of their later joint attention (12 and 18 months). The sample (n = 325), drawn from the Maternal Lifestyle Study, a longitudinal multicenter project conducted at 4 centers of the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Neonatal Research Network, included high-risk (cocaine-exposed) and matched noncocaine-exposed infants. Hierarchical regressions revealed that infants' attention orienting at 1 month significantly predicted more frequent initiating joint attention at 12 (but not 18) months of age. Social engagement at 4 months predicted initiating joint attention at 18 months. Results provide the first empirical evidence for the role of visual attention and social engagement behaviors as developmental precursors for later joint attention outcome. (PsycINFO Database Record

[1]  John Colombo,et al.  Conceptualizing Social Attention in Developmental Research. , 2016, Social development.

[2]  P. Mundy,et al.  Joint Attention, Social Competence, and Developmental Psychopathology , 2015 .

[3]  Sarah Shultz,et al.  Social visual engagement in infants and toddlers with autism: Early developmental transitions and a model of pathogenesis , 2015, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews.

[4]  Lori-Ann R. Sacrey,et al.  Impairments to visual disengagement in autism spectrum disorder: A review of experimental studies from infancy to adulthood , 2014, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[5]  B. Lester,et al.  Assessment and Evaluation of the High Risk Neonate: The NICU Network Neurobehavioral Scale , 2014, Journal of visualized experiments : JoVE.

[6]  D. Messinger,et al.  Joint Attention Initiation With and Without Positive Affect: Risk Group Differences and Associations with ASD Symptoms , 2013, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.

[7]  B. Rogé,et al.  Visual social attention in autism spectrum disorder: Insights from eye tracking studies , 2014, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[8]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Joint Attention without Gaze Following: Human Infants and Their Parents Coordinate Visual Attention to Objects through Eye-Hand Coordination , 2013, PloS one.

[9]  H. Roeyers,et al.  Can infants' orientation to social stimuli predict later joint attention skills? , 2012, The British journal of developmental psychology.

[10]  A. Mastergeorge,et al.  A parallel and distributed‐processing model of joint attention, social cognition and autism , 2009, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.

[11]  A. Engel,et al.  Neuronal Synchronization along the Dorsal Visual Pathway Reflects the Focus of Spatial Attention , 2008, Neuron.

[12]  Mark Sheskin,et al.  Visual disengagement in the infant siblings of children with an autism spectrum disorder (ASD) , 2008, Autism : the international journal of research and practice.

[13]  Louis A. Schmidt,et al.  The “dual usage problem” in the explanations of “joint attention” and children’s socioemotional development: A reconceptualization , 2008 .

[14]  Morton Ann Gernsbacher,et al.  On Privileging the Role of Gaze in Infant Social Cognition. , 2008, Child development perspectives.

[15]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Understanding the referential nature of looking: Infants’ preference for object-directed gaze , 2008, Cognition.

[16]  P. Mundy,et al.  CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Attention, Joint Attention, and Social Cognition , 2022 .

[17]  Brenda Salley,et al.  Temperamental and Joint Attentional Predictors of Language Development , 2007, Merrill-Palmer quarterly.

[18]  P. Mundy,et al.  Individual differences and the development of joint attention in infancy. , 2007, Child development.

[19]  J. Werker,et al.  Listening to language at birth: evidence for a bias for speech in neonates. , 2007, Developmental science.

[20]  J. Richards,et al.  Infants' attention to patterned stimuli: developmental change from 3 to 12 months of age. , 2006, Child development.

[21]  L. Wright,et al.  Cocaine exposure is associated with subtle compromises of infants' and mothers' social-emotional behavior and dyadic features of their interaction in the face-to-face still-face paradigm. , 2005, Developmental psychology.

[22]  T. Striano,et al.  Coordinated affect with mothers and strangers: A longitudinal analysis of joint engagement between 5 and 9 months of age , 2005 .

[23]  David I. Perrett,et al.  An fMRI study of joint attention experience , 2005, NeuroImage.

[24]  P. Mundy,et al.  Infant joint attention skill and preschool behavioral outcomes in at-risk children , 2004, Development and Psychopathology.

[25]  W. Poole,et al.  The maternal lifestyle study: cognitive, motor, and behavioral outcomes of cocaine-exposed and opiate-exposed infants through three years of age. , 2004, Pediatrics.

[26]  B. Lester,et al.  The Neonatal Intensive Care Unit Network Neurobehavioral Scale procedures. , 2004, Pediatrics.

[27]  A. Neal,et al.  Child, Caregiver, and Temperament Contributions to Infant Joint Attention , 2003 .

[28]  P. Mundy Annotation: the neural basis of social impairments in autism: the role of the dorsal medial-frontal cortex and anterior cingulate system. , 2003, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[29]  H. Ruff,et al.  Development of attention and distractibility in the first 4 years of life. , 2003, Developmental psychology.

[30]  P. Mundy,et al.  EEG coherence, joint attention and language development in the second year , 2003 .

[31]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  The importance of eyes: how infants interpret adult looking behavior. , 2002, Developmental psychology.

[32]  M. Yale,et al.  Getting the point: electrophysiological correlates of protodeclarative pointing , 2002, International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience.

[33]  P. Mundy,et al.  Joint attention and disorganized attachment status in infants at risk , 2002, Development and Psychopathology.

[34]  G. Dawson,et al.  Neurocognitive function and joint attention ability in young children with autism spectrum disorder versus developmental delay. , 2002, Child development.

[35]  L. Wright,et al.  The Maternal Lifestyle Study: drug exposure during pregnancy and short-term maternal outcomes. , 2002, American journal of obstetrics and gynecology.

[36]  J. Martinerie,et al.  The brainweb: Phase synchronization and large-scale integration , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[37]  J. Richards Cortical Indexes of Saccade Planning Following Covert Orienting in 20-Week-Old Infants , 2001 .

[38]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Differential Frontal Cortex Activation Before Anticipatory and Reactive Saccades in Infants , 2001 .

[39]  I. Bushnell,et al.  Mother's face recognition in newborn infants: learning and memory , 2001 .

[40]  L. Wright,et al.  The Maternal Lifestyle Study: drug use by meconium toxicology and maternal self-report. , 2001, Pediatrics.

[41]  N. Emery,et al.  The eyes have it: the neuroethology, function and evolution of social gaze , 2000, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[42]  P. Mundy,et al.  EEG correlates of the development of infant joint attention skills. , 2000, Developmental psychobiology.

[43]  Michael Morales,et al.  Responding to Joint Attention Across the 6- Through 24-Month Age Period and Early Language Acquisition , 2000 .

[44]  Michael Morales,et al.  Gaze following, temperament, and language development in 6-month-olds: A replication and extension , 2000 .

[45]  P. Rochat,et al.  Developmental link between dyadic and triadic social competence in infancy , 1999 .

[46]  B. Lester,et al.  The NICU Network Neurobehavioral Scale. Clinical use with drug exposed infants and their mothers. , 1999, Clinics in perinatology.

[47]  L. Lagasse,et al.  Cocaine Exposure and Children: The Meaning of Subtle Effects , 1998, Science.

[48]  S. E. Ulvund,et al.  The predictive validity of nonverbal communicative skills in infants with perinatal hazards , 1996 .

[49]  C. Umilta,et al.  Face preference at birth. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[50]  M. Rothbart,et al.  Attention in Early Development: Themes and Variations , 1996 .

[51]  P. Mundy Joint attention and social-emotional approach behavior in children with autism , 1995, Development and Psychopathology.

[52]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting in normal and pathological development , 1994, Development and Psychopathology.

[53]  Connie Kasari,et al.  Joint attention, developmental level, and symptom presentation in autism , 1994, Development and Psychopathology.

[54]  William P. Fifer,et al.  Two-day-olds prefer their native language , 1993 .

[55]  Connie Kasari,et al.  Nonverbal communication, affective sharing, and intersubjectivity , 1992 .

[56]  R. Aslin,et al.  Preference for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth. , 1990, Child development.

[57]  P. Mundy,et al.  Affective sharing in the context of joint attention interactions of normal, autistic, and mentally retarded children , 1990, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[58]  E. Tronick Emotions and emotional communication in infants. , 1989, The American psychologist.

[59]  P. Mundy,et al.  Defining the social deficits of autism: the contribution of non-verbal communication measures. , 1986, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[60]  L. Adamson,et al.  Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother-infant and peer-infant interaction. , 1984, Child development.

[61]  A. Decasper,et al.  Of Human Bonding: Newborns Prefer Their Mothers' Voices , 1980 .

[62]  L. Sroufe,et al.  The Coherence of Individual Development: Early Care, Attachment, and Subsequent Developmental Issues. , 1979 .

[63]  T. Brazelton,et al.  The infant's response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction. , 1978, Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry.

[64]  J. Bruner,et al.  The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant , 1975, Nature.

[65]  Jerome S. Bruner,et al.  Education as Social Invention , 1964 .

[66]  P. Mundy,et al.  Neural systems and the development of gaze-following and related joint attention skills , 2017 .

[67]  P. Mundy,et al.  Anticipatory smiling: linking early affective communication and social outcome. , 2009, Infant behavior & development.

[68]  D. J. Shaddy,et al.  The Developmental Course of Habituation in Infancy and Preschool Outcome , 2004 .

[69]  J. Colombo The development of visual attention in infancy. , 2001, Annual review of psychology.

[70]  C. Trevarthen,et al.  Infant intersubjectivity: research, theory, and clinical applications. , 2001, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[71]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. , 1998, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[72]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[73]  C. Dweck Self-theories and goals: their role in motivation, personality, and development. , 1990, Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation.

[74]  J. Werker,et al.  Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life , 1984 .

[75]  L. Yarrow Assessment of mastery motivation during the first year of life: Contemporaneous and cross-age relationships. , 1983 .