Privileged versus shared knowledge about object identity in real-time referential processing

A central claim in research on interactive conversation is that listeners use the knowledge assumed to be shared with a conversational partner to guide their understanding of utterances from the earliest moments of processing. In the present study we investigated whether this claim extends to cases where shared vs. private knowledge is discrepant in terms of the identity assigned to a mutually seen object that could be misidentified on the basis of its appearance. Eye movement measures were used to evaluate listeners' ability to integrate a speaker's perspective as they identified the referent for an unfolding expression. The results reconfirmed previous findings showing that listeners can rapidly take into account a speaker's awareness of the existence/presence of a referential object. In contrast, however, listeners showed strong consideration of their private knowledge about the identity of an object during referential processing. Strikingly, this tendency was found even when speaker-produced discourse reinforced the way in which the speaker's understanding of the object's identity differed from that of the listener. Together, the results reveal clear and important differences in the way in which distinct types of perspective-based cues are integrated in real-time communicative interaction.

[1]  Herbert H. Clark,et al.  Grounding in communication , 1991, Perspectives on socially shared cognition.

[2]  Julie C. Sedivy,et al.  Evidence of Perspective-Taking Constraints in Children's On-Line Reference Resolution , 2002, Psychological science.

[3]  J. Perner,et al.  Opacity and Discourse Referents: Object Identity and Object Properties , 2007 .

[4]  S. Gelman Psychological essentialism in children , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  D. Barr Pragmatic expectations and linguistic evidence: Listeners anticipate but do not integrate common ground , 2008, Cognition.

[6]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  The importance of eyes: how infants interpret adult looking behavior. , 2002, Developmental psychology.

[7]  Sarah Brown-Schmidt,et al.  Addressees distinguish shared from private information when interpreting questions during interactive conversation , 2008, Cognition.

[8]  D. Barr,et al.  Perspective-free pragmatics: Broken precedents and the recovery-from-preemption hypothesis , 2007 .

[9]  Susan Brennan,et al.  Partner-Specific Adaptation in Dialog , 2009, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[10]  N. Epley,et al.  Reflexively mindblind: Using theory of mind to interpret behavior requires effortful attention , 2010 .

[11]  M. Tanenhaus,et al.  The effects of common ground and perspective on domains of referential interpretation , 2003 .

[12]  D. Barr,et al.  Taking Perspective in Conversation: The Role of Mutual Knowledge in Comprehension , 2000, Psychological science.

[13]  Stanford Universi Common Ground and the Understanding of Demonstrative Reference , 2004 .

[14]  M. Tanenhaus,et al.  The role of perspective in identifying domains of reference , 2008, Cognition.

[15]  D. Barr,et al.  The Egocentric Basis of Language Use , 1998 .

[16]  Heather J. Ferguson,et al.  Eye movements reveal the time-course of anticipating behaviour based on complex, conflicting desires , 2011, Cognition.

[17]  D. Barr,et al.  Limits on theory of mind use in adults , 2003, Cognition.

[18]  John H. Flavell,et al.  The development of children's knowledge about the mind: From cognitive connections to mental representations. , 1988 .

[19]  J. Perner,et al.  Opacity and embedded perspectives: object identity and object properties , 2007 .

[20]  Paul D. Allopenna,et al.  Tracking the Time Course of Spoken Word Recognition Using Eye Movements: Evidence for Continuous Mapping Models , 1998 .

[21]  M. Tanenhaus,et al.  Continuous mapping from sound to meaning in spoken-language comprehension: immediate effects of verb-based thematic constraints. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[22]  E. Robinson,et al.  Children's difficulties handling dual identity. , 2001, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[23]  Uta Frith,et al.  Theory of mind , 2001, Current Biology.

[24]  L. Kaufman,et al.  Handbook of perception and human performance , 1986 .

[25]  D. Olson,et al.  Children's Representational Theory of Language:The Problem of Opaque Contexts , 1999 .

[26]  Rechele Brooks,et al.  Gaze following at 12 and 14 months: Do the eyes matter? , 2002 .

[27]  H. Wellman,et al.  Insides and essences: Early understandings of the non-obvious , 1991, Cognition.

[28]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Level 1 perspective-taking at 24 months of age , 2006 .

[29]  S. Brown-Schmidt,et al.  Partner-specific interpretation of maintained referential precedents during interactive dialog. , 2009, Journal of memory and language.

[30]  R. Gibbs,et al.  Common Ground in Asking and Understanding Questions , 1988, Language and speech.

[31]  H. H. Clark,et al.  Conceptual pacts and lexical choice in conversation. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[32]  D. Barr,et al.  Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. , 2013, Journal of memory and language.

[33]  H. H. Clark,et al.  Common ground at the understanding of demonstrative reference , 1983 .

[34]  Kirk Ludwig,et al.  Semantics for Opaque Contexts , 1998 .

[35]  D. Barr Analyzing ‘visual world’ eyetracking data using multilevel logistic regression , 2008 .

[36]  Stephanie D. Teasley,et al.  Perspectives on socially shared cognition , 1991 .

[37]  Michael L. Littman,et al.  Disambiguation by community membership , 1990, Memory & cognition.

[38]  Ian A Apperly,et al.  Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states? , 2009, Psychological review.

[39]  Michael K. Tanenhaus,et al.  Pragmatic effects on reference resolution in a collaborative task: evidence from eye movements , 2004, Cogn. Sci..

[40]  M. Pickering,et al.  Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[41]  F. Keil Concepts, Kinds, and Cognitive Development , 1989 .

[42]  E. Robinson,et al.  When can children handle referential opacity? Evidence for systematic variation in 5- and 6-year-old children's reasoning about beliefs and belief reports. , 2003, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[43]  E. Markman,et al.  Categories and induction in young children , 1986, Cognition.

[44]  S. Brown-Schmidt,et al.  Beyond common and privileged: Gradient representations of common ground in real-time language use , 2012 .

[45]  O. Mimura [Eye movements]. , 1992, Nippon Ganka Gakkai zasshi.

[46]  Catherine Marshall,et al.  Reference Diaries , 1978, TINLAP.

[47]  Timothy M. Gann,et al.  Anticipatory baseline effects and information integration in visual world studies. , 2011, Acta psychologica.

[48]  D. Muir,et al.  Three-year-olds' difficulty with the appearance--reality distinction: is it real or is it apparent? , 2000, Developmental psychology.

[49]  D. Wood,et al.  Six-year-olds' difficulties handling intensional contexts , 2003, Cognition.

[50]  Craig G. Chambers,et al.  Lexical competition during second-language listening: sentence context, but not proficiency, constrains interference from the native lexicon. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[51]  Michael V. Lombardo,et al.  Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from developmental social neuroscience , 2013 .

[52]  Lawrence D. Roberts How Reference Works: Explanatory Models for Indexicals, Descriptions, and Opacity , 1993 .

[53]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[54]  Joseph Watts,et al.  Attributing False Beliefs About Object Identity Reveals a Signature Blind Spot in Humans’ Efficient Mind-Reading System , 2013, Psychological science.

[55]  S. Frisson,et al.  Psychological essentialist reasoning and perspective taking during reading: A donkey is not a zebra, but a plate can be a clock , 2011, Memory & Cognition.

[56]  D. Davidson On Saying That , 1968 .

[57]  B. Webber,et al.  Elements of Discourse Understanding , 1983 .

[58]  Susan R. Fussell,et al.  The effects of intended audience on message production and comprehension: Reference in a common ground framework , 1989 .

[59]  I. Apperly Mindreaders: The Cognitive Basis of "Theory of Mind" , 2010 .

[60]  B. Sodian,et al.  Now I see it but you don't: 14-month-olds can represent another person's visual perspective. , 2007, Developmental science.

[61]  R. Breheny,et al.  Listeners' eyes reveal spontaneous sensitivity to others' perspectives , 2012 .