EEG beta desynchronization during hand goal-directed action observation in newborn monkeys and its relation to the emergence of hand motor skills
暂无分享,去创建一个
Fabrizia Festante | Ross E. Vanderwert | Nathan A. Fox | Pier Francesco Ferrari | Annika Paukner | Stephen J. Suomi | N. Fox | S. Suomi | P. Ferrari | A. Paukner | Valentina Sclafani | E. Simpson | R. Vanderwert | F. Festante | Valentina Sclafani | Elizabeth A. Simpson | V. Sclafani | Fabrizia Festante
[1] Ross E. Vanderwert,et al. Distinct EEG Amplitude Suppression to Facial Gestures as Evidence for a Mirror Mechanism in Newborn Monkeys , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[2] J. Sommerville,et al. Action experience alters 3-month-old infants' perception of others' actions , 2005, Cognition.
[3] G. Rizzolatti,et al. The Dynamics of Sensorimotor Cortical Oscillations during the Observation of Hand Movements: An EEG Study , 2012, PloS one.
[4] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.
[5] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Parietal Lobe: From Action Organization to Intention Understanding , 2005, Science.
[6] N. Fox,et al. Development of the EEG from 5 months to 4 years of age , 2002, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[7] R. Lemon. Neural control of dexterity: what has been achieved? , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.
[8] C. Heyes. Where do mirror neurons come from? , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[9] Monica Maranesi,et al. Anatomo‐functional organization of the ventral primary motor and premotor cortex in the macaque monkey , 2012, The European journal of neuroscience.
[10] Kathryn H. Yoo,et al. Assessing human mirror activity with EEG mu rhythm: A meta-analysis. , 2016, Psychological bulletin.
[11] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Functional Organization of Inferior Parietal Lobule Convexity in the Macaque Monkey: Electrophysiological Characterization of Motor, Sensory and Mirror Responses and Their Correlation with Cytoarchitectonic Areas , 2022 .
[12] Harold Bekkering,et al. What are you doing? How active and observational experience shape infants' action understanding , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[13] C. Hofsten. An action perspective on motor development , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[14] Andrew N Meltzoff,et al. Neural mirroring mechanisms and imitation in human infants , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[15] Pier Francesco Ferrari,et al. Faces in the mirror, from the neuroscience of mimicry to the emergence of mentalizing. , 2016, Journal of anthropological sciences = Rivista di antropologia : JASS.
[16] Ross E. Vanderwert,et al. Action Experience, More than Observation, Influences Mu Rhythm Desynchronization , 2014, PloS one.
[17] Mark H. Johnson,et al. Motor System Activation Reveals Infants’ On-Line Prediction of Others’ Goals , 2010, Psychological science.
[18] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.
[19] G. Rizzolatti,et al. The Cortical Motor System , 2001, Neuron.
[20] A. Iriki,et al. Mirror neurons through the lens of epigenetics , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[21] J. Pineda. The functional significance of mu rhythms: Translating “seeing” and “hearing” into “doing” , 2005, Brain Research Reviews.
[22] P. Rochat,et al. Development of sitting and reaching in 5- to 6-month-old infants☆ , 1995 .
[23] Kathryn H. Yoo,et al. Desynchronization in EEG during perception of means-end actions and relations with infants' grasping skill. , 2016, The British journal of developmental psychology.
[24] V. Caggiano,et al. The Mirror Neuron System , 2011, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.
[25] S. Suomi,et al. Neonatal imitation and early social experience predict gaze following abilities in infant monkeys , 2016, Scientific Reports.
[26] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Action recognition in the premotor cortex. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[27] Harold Bekkering,et al. Short-term Motor Training, but Not Observational Training, Alters Neurocognitive Mechanisms of Action Processing in Infancy , 2015, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[28] A. Meltzoff,et al. Neural mirroring systems: Exploring the EEG mu rhythm in human infancy , 2011, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.
[29] Marco Bimbi,et al. Simultaneous scalp recorded EEG and local field potentials from monkey ventral premotor cortex during action observation and execution reveals the contribution of mirror and motor neurons to the mu-rhythm , 2018, NeuroImage.
[30] S. Suomi,et al. Neonatal imitation predicts infant rhesus macaque (Macaca mulatta) social and anxiety-related behaviours at one year , 2016, Scientific Reports.
[31] C. Von Hofsten. An action perspective on motor development. , 2004, Trends in cognitive sciences.
[32] C. Heyes,et al. Mirror neurons: from origin to function. , 2014, The Behavioral and brain sciences.
[33] S. Suomi,et al. Interindividual differences in neonatal imitation and the development of action chains in rhesus macaques. , 2009, Child development.
[34] Mark H Johnson,et al. Predictive motor activation during action observation in human infants , 2009, Biology Letters.
[35] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Mirror neurons responding to the observation of ingestive and communicative mouth actions in the monkey ventral premotor cortex , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.
[36] P. Ferrari,et al. Mirror neurons in the tree of life: mosaic evolution, plasticity and exaptation of sensorimotor matching responses , 2017, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.
[37] Cecilia Heyes,et al. A new approach to mirror neurons: Developmental history, system-level theory and intervention experiments , 2013, Cortex.
[38] K. Sugi,et al. [Electrophysiological study]. , 1996, Nihon rinsho. Japanese journal of clinical medicine.
[39] Leonardo Fogassi,et al. Mirror Neurons Responding to Observation of Actions Made with Tools in Monkey Ventral Premotor Cortex , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[40] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[41] Alexander Kraskov,et al. M1 Corticospinal Mirror Neurons and Their Role in Movement Suppression during Action Observation , 2013, Current Biology.
[42] P. Ferrari,et al. The mirror neuron system as revealed through neonatal imitation: presence from birth, predictive power and evidence of plasticity , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[43] Kerstin Rosander,et al. Using mu rhythm desynchronization to measure mirror neuron activity in infants. , 2011, Developmental science.
[44] R. Hari,et al. Human cortical oscillations: a neuromagnetic view through the skull , 1997, Trends in Neurosciences.
[45] Sarah A. Gerson,et al. Mirroring and the development of action understanding , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[46] Eliza L. Nelson,et al. Head orientation and handedness trajectory in rhesus monkey infants (Macaca mulatta). , 2011, Developmental psychobiology.
[47] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[48] A. Woodward,et al. Relations between infants' emerging reach-grasp competence and event-related desynchronization in EEG. , 2016, Developmental science.
[49] Pier Francesco Ferrari,et al. Development of space perception in relation to the maturation of the motor system in infant rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) , 2015, Neuropsychologia.
[50] Hein T. van Schie,et al. You'll never crawl alone: Neurophysiological evidence for experience-dependent motor resonance in infancy , 2008, NeuroImage.
[51] G. Pfurtscheller,et al. Event-related dynamics of cortical rhythms: frequency-specific features and functional correlates. , 2001, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[52] Francesca Ugolotti Serventi,et al. Ventral premotor and inferior parietal cortices make distinct contribution to action organization and intention understanding. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.
[53] N. Fox,et al. Frequency and topography in monkey electroencephalogram during action observation: possible neural correlates of the mirror neuron system , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[54] Febo Cincotti,et al. Human Cortical Electroencephalography (EEG) Rhythms during the Observation of Simple Aimless Movements: A High-Resolution EEG Study , 2002, NeuroImage.
[55] Etienne Olivier,et al. An Electrophysiological Study of the Postnatal Development of the Corticospinal System in the Macaque Monkey , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[56] L. Oberman,et al. What goads cigarette smokers to smoke? Neural adaptation and the mirror neuron system , 2006, Brain Research.
[57] N. Fox,et al. The mirror mechanism and mu rhythm in social development , 2013, Neuroscience Letters.
[58] Ross E. Vanderwert,et al. Early Social Experience Affects Neural Activity to Affiliative Facial Gestures in Newborn Nonhuman Primates , 2015, Developmental Neuroscience.
[59] I. Darian‐Smith,et al. Postnatal maturation of the direct corticospinal projections in the macaque monkey. , 1995, Cerebral cortex.
[60] Blake W. Johnson,et al. Changes in rolandic mu rhythm during observation of a precision grip. , 2004, Psychophysiology.
[61] M. Del Giudice,et al. Programmed to learn? The ontogeny of mirror neurons. , 2009, Developmental science.
[62] N. Fox,et al. Spectral and source structural development of mu and alpha rhythms from infancy through adulthood , 2016, Clinical Neurophysiology.