Subthreshold muscle twitches dissociate oscillatory neural signatures of conflicts from errors

We investigated the neural systems underlying conflict detection and error monitoring during rapid online error correction/monitoring mechanisms. We combined data from four separate cognitive tasks and 64 subjects in which EEG and EMG (muscle activity from the thumb used to respond) were recorded. In typical neuroscience experiments, behavioral responses are classified as "error" or "correct"; however, closer inspection of our data revealed that correct responses were often accompanied by "partial errors" - a muscle twitch of the incorrect hand ("mixed correct trials," ~13% of the trials). We found that these muscle twitches dissociated conflicts from errors in time-frequency domain analyses of EEG data. In particular, both mixed-correct trials and full error trials were associated with enhanced theta-band power (4-9Hz) compared to correct trials. However, full errors were additionally associated with power and frontal-parietal synchrony in the delta band. Single-trial robust multiple regression analyses revealed a significant modulation of theta power as a function of partial error correction time, thus linking trial-to-trial fluctuations in power to conflict. Furthermore, single-trial correlation analyses revealed a qualitative dissociation between conflict and error processing, such that mixed correct trials were associated with positive theta-RT correlations whereas full error trials were associated with negative delta-RT correlations. These findings shed new light on the local and global network mechanisms of conflict monitoring and error detection, and their relationship to online action adjustment.

[1]  Dénes Szücs,et al.  Motor conflict in Stroop tasks: Direct evidence from single-trial electro-myography and electro-encephalography , 2009, NeuroImage.

[2]  Anna Weinberg,et al.  Biological Psychology , 2022 .

[3]  Sidney J. Segalowitz,et al.  The error-related negativity associated with different strength of stimulus–response interference , 2012, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[4]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Theta Dynamics Reveal Domain-specific Control over Stimulus and Response Conflict , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[5]  Borís Burle,et al.  Executive control in the Simon effect: an electromyographic and distributional analysis , 2002, Psychological research.

[6]  B. Burle,et al.  Error negativity on correct trials: a reexamination of available data , 2003, Biological Psychology.

[7]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of anterior cingulate function in a go/no-go task: Effects of response conflict and trial type frequency , 2003, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[8]  M. Rosenberg,et al.  In the zone or zoning out? Tracking behavioral and neural fluctuations during sustained attention. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[9]  John J. B. Allen,et al.  Theta EEG dynamics of the error-related negativity , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[10]  W. Singer,et al.  Modulation of Neuronal Interactions Through Neuronal Synchronization , 2007, Science.

[11]  Franck Vidal,et al.  Sequential adjustments before and after partial errors , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[12]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Error-related brain potentials are differentially related to awareness of response errors: evidence from an antisaccade task. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[13]  M. Girelli,et al.  Does subliminal visual perception have an error‐monitoring system? , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.

[14]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Not All Errors Are Alike: Theta and Alpha EEG Dynamics Relate to Differences in Error-Processing Dynamics , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Dynamic Interactions between Large-Scale Brain Networks Predict Behavioral Adaptation after Perceptual Errors , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[16]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Errors of interpretation and modeling: A reply to Grinband et al. , 2011, NeuroImage.

[17]  Sabine Kastner,et al.  Functional heterogeneity of conflict, error, task-switching, and unexpectedness effects within medial prefrontal cortex , 2011, NeuroImage.

[18]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Distinct Brain Mechanisms for Conscious versus Subliminal Error Detection , 2022 .

[19]  Dianne P. O’LEARYt ROBUST REGRESSION COMPUTATION USING ITERATIVELY REWEIGHTED LEAST SQUARES * , 2022 .

[20]  F. Varela,et al.  Measuring phase synchrony in brain signals , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[21]  John J. B. Allen,et al.  Theta lingua franca: a common mid-frontal substrate for action monitoring processes. , 2012, Psychophysiology.

[22]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[23]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Reply to “Higher response time increases theta energy, conflict increases response time” , 2013, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[24]  M. Coles,et al.  Performance monitoring in a confusing world: error-related brain activity, judgments of response accuracy, and types of errors. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Error-related medial frontal theta activity predicts cingulate-related structural connectivity , 2011, NeuroImage.

[26]  Simon Hanslmayr,et al.  The Electrophysiological Dynamics of Interference during the Stroop Task , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  A. Turken,et al.  Dissociation between conflict detection and error monitoring in the human anterior cingulate cortex , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[28]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Unconscious Errors Enhance Prefrontal-Occipital Oscillatory Synchrony , 2009, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[29]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Why is there an ERN/Ne on correct trials? Response representations, stimulus-related components, and the theory of error-processing , 2001, Biological Psychology.

[30]  C. Carter,et al.  Error Detection, Correction, and Prevention in the Brain: A Brief Review of Data and Theories , 2006, Clinical EEG and neuroscience.

[31]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  It's about Time , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci.

[32]  Thérèse J. M. Overbeek,et al.  Dissociable Components of Error Processing on the Functional Significance of the Pe Vis-à-vis the Ern/ne Performance Monitoring Processes Reflected in the Ne and Pe Review of Studies That Report Both Ne and Pe: Associations and Dissociations Pharmacological Effects , 2022 .

[33]  J. Ford,et al.  Anatomy of an error: ERP and fMRI , 2003, Biological Psychology.

[34]  M. Falkenstein,et al.  Does the error negativity reflect the degree of response conflict? , 2006, Brain Research.

[35]  P. Nachev Cognition and medial frontal cortex in health and disease , 2006, Current opinion in neurology.

[36]  T. Endrass,et al.  Overactive performance monitoring in obsessive-compulsive disorder: ERP evidence from correct and erroneous reactions , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[37]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.

[38]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Midfrontal conflict-related theta-band power reflects neural oscillations that predict behavior. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: A primer with examples , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[40]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  ERP components on reaction errors and their functional significance: a tutorial , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[41]  Stefan Scherbaum,et al.  Higher response time increases theta energy, conflict increases response time , 2013, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[42]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Single-Trial Regression Elucidates the Role of Prefrontal Theta Oscillations in Response Conflict , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[43]  S. Taylor,et al.  Conditional Differences in Mean Reaction Time Explain Effects of Response Congruency, but not Accuracy, on Posterior Medial Frontal Cortex Activity , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[44]  T. Egner Congruency sequence effects and cognitive control , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[45]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Neurocognitive mechanisms of cognitive control: The role of prefrontal cortex in action selection, response inhibition, performance monitoring, and reward-based learning , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[46]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[47]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Frontal Oscillatory Dynamics Predict Feedback Learning and Action Adjustment , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[48]  Michael X Cohen,et al.  Analyzing Neural Time Series Data: Theory and Practice , 2014 .

[49]  P. Nunez,et al.  Comparison of the effect of volume conduction on EEG coherence with the effect of field spread on MEG coherence , 2007, Statistics in medicine.

[50]  Kristina M. Visscher,et al.  The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[51]  F. Vidal,et al.  Is the ‘error negativity’ specific to errors? , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[52]  D. O’Leary Robust regression computation computation using iteratively reweighted least squares , 1990 .

[53]  Borís Burle,et al.  Rostral Cingulate Zone and correct response monitoring: ICA and source localization evidences for the unicity of correct- and error-negativities , 2010, NeuroImage.

[54]  D. V. Cramon,et al.  Subprocesses of Performance Monitoring: A Dissociation of Error Processing and Response Competition Revealed by Event-Related fMRI and ERPs , 2001, NeuroImage.

[55]  Jürgen Kayser,et al.  Principal components analysis of Laplacian waveforms as a generic method for identifying ERP generator patterns: I. Evaluation with auditory oddball tasks , 2006, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[56]  Juliana Yordanova,et al.  Parallel systems of error processing in the brain , 2004, NeuroImage.

[57]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. , 2004, Psychological review.

[58]  P. Nunez,et al.  EEG and MEG coherence: Measures of functional connectivity at distinct spatial scales of neocortical dynamics , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[59]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Pre- and poststimulus activation of response channels: a psychophysiological analysis. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[60]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , 2004 .

[61]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  EEG Source Reconstruction Reveals Frontal-Parietal Dynamics of Spatial Conflict Processing , 2013, PloS one.

[62]  M. Coles,et al.  Where did you go wrong? Errors, partial errors, and the nature of human information processing. , 1995, Acta psychologica.

[63]  Eric H Schumacher,et al.  Electromyographic evidence for response conflict in the exclude recognition task , 2009, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[64]  John J. B. Allen,et al.  Prelude to and Resolution of an Error: EEG Phase Synchrony Reveals Cognitive Control Dynamics during Action Monitoring , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[65]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[66]  Borís Burle,et al.  Error Negativity Does Not Reflect Conflict: A Reappraisal of Conflict Monitoring and Anterior Cingulate Cortex Activity , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.