The representation of instructions in working memory leads to autonomous response activation: Evidence from the first trials in the flanker paradigm

The authors examined whether instructions can lead to autonomous response activation even without practice. Eriksen and Eriksen's (1974) flanker compatibility paradigm was used to show that the flanker compatibility effect (FCE) is already present in the first trials following the stimulus–response instructions, before any of the stimuli have been repeated. This first-trials FCE was present even when participants were strongly motivated to ignore the flankers, and it disappeared under conditions of high working-memory (WM) load. The findings suggest that intention, formed by instructions, is involved in forming representations in WM that operate like a prepared reflex (Woodworth, 1938). The implications of the finding to intentionality and frontal lobe functions are discussed.

[1]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[2]  Charles Curtis Eriksen,et al.  The extent of processing of noise elements during selective encoding from visual displays , 1973 .

[3]  Asher Cohen,et al.  Perceptual Dimensional Constraints in Response Selection Processes , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.

[4]  B. Hommel Inverting the Simon effect intention: Determinants of direction and extent of effects of irrelevant spatial information. , 1993 .

[5]  C. Eriksen,et al.  The flankers task and response competition: A useful tool for investigating a variety of cognitive problems , 1995 .

[6]  J. Bargh,et al.  The automated will: nonconscious activation and pursuit of behavioral goals. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[7]  S. Tipper,et al.  Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , 1948, Nature.

[8]  J. Duncan Selective attention and the organization of visual information. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[9]  G. Grice,et al.  Temporal characteristics of noise conditions producing facilitation and interference , 1985, Perception & psychophysics.

[10]  E. Fox Negative priming from ignored distractors in visual selection: A review , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[11]  M. Zorzi,et al.  The role of long-term-memory and short-term-memory links in the Simon effect. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[12]  R W Remington,et al.  The structure of attentional control: contingent attentional capture by apparent motion, abrupt onset, and color. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A parallel distributed processing approach to automaticity. , 1992, The American journal of psychology.

[14]  G. Grice,et al.  Temporal dynamics of associative interference and facilitation produced by visual context , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.

[15]  Charles L. Folk,et al.  A Dissociation Between Attention and Selection , 2001, Psychological science.

[16]  N. Ach,et al.  Uber den Willensakt und das Temperament. , 1910 .

[17]  D. Kahneman,et al.  The reviewing of object files: Object-specific integration of information , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.

[18]  Gordon D Logan,et al.  An instance theory of attention and memory. , 2002, Psychological review.

[19]  Peter A. Frensch,et al.  The Role of Information Reduction in Skill Acquisition , 1996, Cognitive Psychology.

[20]  J H Flowers,et al.  The effect of flanking context on visual classification: The joint contribution of interactions at different processing levels , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.

[21]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  On the locus of visual selection: evidence from focused attention tasks. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[22]  J. Duncan,et al.  Visual search and stimulus similarity. , 1989, Psychological review.

[23]  S. Wise,et al.  Rule-dependent neuronal activity in the prefrontal cortex , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Early, involuntary top-down guidance of attention from working memory. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  J. Pratt,et al.  Symbolic control of visual attention: The role of working memory and attentional control settings. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  K. Oberauer Access to information in working memory: exploring the focus of attention. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[27]  J. Tzelgov,et al.  Automatic comparisons of artificial digits never compared: learning linear ordering relations. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[28]  J. R. Simon The Effects of an Irrelevant Directional CUE on Human Information Processing , 1990 .

[29]  S. Yantis,et al.  On the distinction between visual salience and stimulus-driven attentional capture. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  H. Egeth,et al.  Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[31]  J. Fuster Memory in the cerebral cortex , 1994 .

[32]  Hilde Haider,et al.  Information reduction during skill acquisition: The influence of task instruction , 1999 .

[33]  Charles W. Eriksen,et al.  Target redundancy in visual search: Do repetitions of the target within thedisplay impair processing? , 1979 .

[34]  Bruce Milliken,et al.  Negative priming in a spatial localization task: feature mismatching and distractor inhibition , 1994 .

[35]  M. Coltheart,et al.  The quarterly journal of experimental psychology , 1985 .

[36]  K. Oberauer Removing irrelevant information from working memory: a cognitive aging study with the modified Sternberg task. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[37]  L Harms,et al.  Color segregation and selective attention in a nonsearch task , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[38]  N. Ach,et al.  Über den Willensakt und das Temperament : eine experimentelle Untersuchung , 1910 .

[39]  James J. Gibson,et al.  A critical review of the concept of set in contemporary experimental psychology. , 1941 .

[40]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[41]  R. Thouless Experimental Psychology , 1939, Nature.

[42]  M. Eimer,et al.  Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[43]  B Hommel,et al.  The role of attention for the Simon effect , 1993, Psychological research.

[44]  Lynn Hasher,et al.  Determinants of Negative Priming , 1995 .

[45]  Robert Nullmeyer,et al.  Application of variable criterion theory to choice reaction time , 1977 .

[46]  L. Jacoby,et al.  Spontaneous Versus Directed Recognition : The Relativity of Automaticity , 2004 .

[47]  N. Cowan Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human information-processing system. , 1988, Psychological bulletin.

[48]  B. Hommel The prepared reflex: Automaticity and control in stimulus-response translation , 2000 .

[49]  P. Barber,et al.  Interference effects in the Stroop and Simon paradigms. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[50]  R. Proctor,et al.  Processing of an irrelevant location dimension as a function of the relevant stimulus dimension. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[51]  C. L. M. The Psychology of Attention , 1890, Nature.

[52]  R. Remington,et al.  Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: evidence for two forms of attentional capture. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[53]  A. Treisman,et al.  Binding in short-term visual memory. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[54]  J. Bargh Conditional automaticity: Varieties of automatic influence in social perception and cognition. , 1989 .

[55]  Gordon D. Logan,et al.  Attention in Character-Classification Tasks : Evidence for the Automaticity of Component Stages , 1978 .

[56]  C W Eriksen,et al.  Information processing in visual search: A continuous flow conception and experimental results , 1979, Perception & psychophysics.

[57]  J. Hoffmann,et al.  Conscious control over the content of unconscious cognition , 2003, Cognition.

[58]  E. Viding,et al.  Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[59]  A. Kramer,et al.  Perceptual organization and focused attention: The role of objects and proximity in visual processing , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[60]  Jeff F. Miller The flanker compatibility effect as a function of visual angle, attentional focus, visual transients, and perceptual load: A search for boundary conditions , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[61]  A. Henik,et al.  Automaticity and consciousness: is perceiving the word necessary for reading it? , 1997, The American journal of psychology.

[62]  Robert W. Proctor,et al.  Stimulus-Response Compatibility: An Integrated Perspective , 1990 .

[63]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Contingent attentional capture: A reply to Yantis (1993). , 1993 .

[64]  G. Logan Attention in character-classification tasks: Evidence for the automaticity of component stages. , 1978 .

[65]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  Task-set reconfiguration with predictable and unpredictable task switches , 2003, Memory & cognition.

[66]  Robert W. Proctor,et al.  Repetition effects with categorizable stimulus and response sets. , 1993 .

[67]  C. Frith,et al.  The Role of Working Memory in Visual Selective Attention , 2001, Science.

[68]  C. Eriksen,et al.  A psychophysiological investigation of the continuous flow model of human information processing. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[69]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[70]  Y Tsal,et al.  A salient distractor does not disrupt conjunction search , 1999, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[71]  J. Bargh,et al.  Environmental control of goal-directed action: automatic and strategic contingencies between situations and behavior. , 1994, Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation.

[72]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[73]  R. Gaschler,et al.  Instruction-induced feature binding , 2007, Psychological research.

[74]  P. Frensch,et al.  The influence of task instruction on action coding: constraint setting or direct coding? , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[75]  H. Munk Zur Physiologie der Grosshirnrinde , 1902 .

[76]  Richard L. Abrams,et al.  Subliminal words activate semantic categories (not automated motor responses) , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.