Circular inference in bistable perception
暂无分享,去创建一个
Renaud Jardri | Pantelis Leptourgos | Sophie Denève | R. Jardri | S. Denéve | Charles-Edouard Notredame | Marion Eck | C. Notredame | M. Eck | P. Leptourgos
[1] S. Denéve,et al. Neural processing as causal inference , 2011, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[2] Joel Pearson,et al. Sensory memory for ambiguous vision , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[3] R. Lynn. Reversible perspective as a function of stimulus-intensity. , 1961, American Journal of Psychology.
[4] Michael Okun,et al. Instantaneous correlation of excitation and inhibition during ongoing and sensory-evoked activities , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.
[5] L. A. N. Esq.,et al. LXI. Observations on some remarkable optical phænomena seen in Switzerland; and on an optical phænomenon which occurs on viewing a figure of a crystal or geometrical solid , 1832 .
[6] David A. Leopold,et al. Stable perception of visually ambiguous patterns , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.
[7] C. Clifford,et al. Mechanisms selectively engaged in rivalry: normal vision habituates, rivalrous vision primes , 2005, Vision Research.
[8] Renaud Jardri,et al. Can circular inference relate the neuropathological and behavioral aspects of schizophrenia? , 2017, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[9] R. Sundareswara,et al. Perceptual multistability predicted by search model for Bayesian decisions. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[10] P. Petrovic,et al. Delusions and the Role of Beliefs in Perceptual Inference , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[11] Adrian R. Willoughby,et al. Effects of Working Memory Load on Visual Selective Attention: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[12] P. Dayan,et al. Cortical substrates for exploratory decisions in humans , 2006, Nature.
[13] Patrick Monnier,et al. Searching for variegated elements. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[14] Thomas C. Toppino,et al. Reversible-figure perception: Mechanisms of intentional control , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.
[15] Jan Drugowitsch,et al. Computational Precision of Mental Inference as Critical Source of Human Choice Suboptimality , 2016, Neuron.
[16] Frank Tong,et al. The Functional Impact of Mental Imagery on Conscious Perception , 2008, Current Biology.
[17] Zhe Qu,et al. Specificity of face processing without awareness , 2010, Consciousness and Cognition.
[18] Pascal Mamassian,et al. Temporal dynamics in bistable perception. , 2005, Journal of vision.
[19] M. Landy,et al. Movement planning with probabilistic target information. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.
[20] Konrad Paul Kording,et al. Causal Inference in Multisensory Perception , 2007, PloS one.
[21] Duje Tadin,et al. Understanding Attentional Modulation of Binocular Rivalry: A Framework Based on Biased Competition , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[22] M. Ernst,et al. Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.
[23] Rachel N. Denison,et al. Predictive Context Influences Perceptual Selection during Binocular Rivalry , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[24] Wei Ji Ma,et al. A detailed comparison of optimality and simplicity in perceptual decision making. , 2016, Psychological review.
[25] Randolph Blake,et al. Exogenous attention and endogenous attention influence initial dominance in binocular rivalry , 2006, Vision Research.
[26] D. Knill,et al. Bayesian sampling in visual perception , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[27] Renaud Jardri,et al. Experimental evidence for circular inference in schizophrenia , 2017, Nature Communications.
[28] I Rock,et al. Why do ambiguous figures reverse? , 1994, Acta psychologica.
[29] R. Zemel,et al. Inference and computation with population codes. , 2003, Annual review of neuroscience.
[30] V. Noreika,et al. Interaction of bottom-up and top-down processes in the perception of ambiguous figures , 2013, Vision Research.
[31] J. Saunders,et al. Demonstration of cue recruitment: change in visual appearance by means of Pavlovian conditioning. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[32] A. Pouget,et al. Not Noisy, Just Wrong: The Role of Suboptimal Inference in Behavioral Variability , 2012, Neuron.
[33] Marisa Carrasco,et al. Attention model of binocular rivalry , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[34] Edward H. Adelson,et al. Motion illusions as optimal percepts , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.
[35] C. Wheatstone. XVIII. Contributions to the physiology of vision. —Part the first. On some remarkable, and hitherto unobserved, phenomena of binocular vision , 1962, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London.
[36] L. Standing,et al. Satiation Effects with Reversible Figures , 1981, Perceptual and motor skills.
[37] G. J. Brouwer,et al. Voluntary control and the dynamics of perceptual bi-stability , 2005, Vision Research.
[38] Charles Wheatstone. On some remarkable and hitherto unobserved phenomena of binocular vision. , 1962 .
[39] W. Levelt. The alternation process in binocular rivalry , 1966 .
[40] R. Jardri,et al. Circular inferences in schizophrenia. , 2013, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[41] Joshua B. Tenenbaum,et al. Multistability and Perceptual Inference , 2012, Neural Computation.
[42] A. Yuille,et al. Object perception as Bayesian inference. , 2004, Annual review of psychology.
[43] Alexander Pastukhov,et al. Cumulative history quantifies the role of neural adaptation in multistable perception. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[44] Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al. Perceptual manifestations of fast neural plasticity: Motion priming, rapid motion aftereffect and perceptual sensitization , 2005, Vision Research.
[45] Alexander Pastukhov,et al. The Role of Attention in Ambiguous Reversals of Structure-From-Motion , 2012, PloS one.
[46] J. Orbach,et al. Reversibility of the Necker Cube: I. An Examination of the Concept of “Satiation of Orientation” , 1963, Perceptual and motor skills.
[47] Michael Bach,et al. Multistable perception: When bottom-up and top-down coincide , 2009, Brain and Cognition.
[48] Wei Ji Ma,et al. Neural coding of uncertainty and probability. , 2014, Annual review of neuroscience.
[49] Luigi Acerbi,et al. On the Origins of Suboptimality in Human Probabilistic Inference , 2014, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[50] M Nawrot,et al. Neural integration of information specifying structure from stereopsis and motion. , 1989, Science.
[51] Pascal Mamassian,et al. Observer biases in the 3D interpretation of line drawings , 1998, Vision Research.
[52] David J Heeger,et al. Theory of cortical function , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[53] Arash Yazdanbakhsh,et al. Effect of Visual Cues on the Resolution of Perceptual Ambiguity in Parkinson’s Disease and Normal Aging , 2015, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.
[54] Ying Wang,et al. The interaction of perceptual biases in bistable perception , 2017, Scientific Reports.
[55] K. Shapiro,et al. The contingent negative variation (CNV) event-related potential (ERP) predicts the attentional blink , 2008 .
[56] Thomas C. Toppino,et al. Enduring interest in perceptual ambiguity: alternating views of reversible figures. , 2004, Psychological bulletin.
[57] P. Christiaan Klink,et al. General Validity of Levelt's Propositions Reveals Common Computational Mechanisms for Visual Rivalry , 2008, PloS one.
[58] W. Ma. Organizing probabilistic models of perception , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[59] N. Logothetis,et al. Visual competition , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[60] M. Scanziani,et al. Equalizing Excitation-Inhibition Ratios across Visual Cortical Neurons , 2014, Nature.
[61] Erich W Graf,et al. Natural images dominate in binocular rivalry , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[62] Raymond van Ee,et al. Distributions of alternation rates in various forms of bistable perception. , 2005, Journal of vision.
[63] Allan C. Dobbins,et al. Asymmetries in Perception of 3D Orientation , 2010, PloS one.