Developmental Differences in the Control of Action Selection by Social Information

Our everyday actions are often performed in the context of a social interaction. We previously showed that, in adults, selecting an action on the basis of either social or symbolic cues was associated with activations in the fronto-parietal cognitive control network, whereas the presence and use of social versus symbolic cues was in addition associated with activations in the temporal and medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC) social brain network. Here we investigated developmental changes in these two networks. Fourteen adults (21–30 years of age) and 14 adolescents (11–16 years) followed instructions to move objects in a set of shelves. Interpretation of the instructions was conditional on the point of view of a visible “director” or the meaning of a symbolic cue (Director Present vs. Director Absent) and the number of potential referent objects in the shelves (3-object vs. 1-object). 3-object trials elicited increased fronto-parietal and temporal activations, with greater left lateral prefrontal cortex and parietal activations in adults than adolescents. Social versus symbolic information led to activations in superior dorsal MPFC, precuneus, and along the superior/middle temporal sulci. Both dorsal MPFC and left temporal clusters exhibited a Director × Object interaction, with greater activation when participants needed to consider the directors' viewpoints. This effect differed with age in dorsal MPFC. Adolescents showed greater activation whenever social information was present, whereas adults showed greater activation only when the directors' viewpoints were relevant to task performance. This study thus shows developmental differences in domain-general and domain-specific PFC activations associated with action selection in a social interaction context.

[1]  Bregtje Gunther Moor,et al.  Neurodevelopmental changes of reading the mind in the eyes. , 2012, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[2]  B. Luna,et al.  What has fMRI told us about the Development of Cognitive Control through Adolescence? , 2010, Brain and Cognition.

[3]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  "Who" Is Saying "What"? Brain-Based Decoding of Human Voice and Speech , 2008, Science.

[4]  Dana Samson,et al.  Executive function is necessary for perspective selection, not Level-1 visual perspective calculation: Evidence from a dual-task study of adults , 2010, Cognition.

[5]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Dissociable Medial Prefrontal Contributions to Judgments of Similar and Dissimilar Others , 2006, Neuron.

[6]  D. Barr,et al.  Limits on theory of mind use in adults , 2003, Cognition.

[7]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Human cortical mechanisms of visual attention during orienting and search. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[8]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Repetition suppression of ventromedial prefrontal activity during judgments of self and others , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[9]  Sarah-Jayne Blakemore,et al.  Is adolescence a sensitive period for sociocultural processing? , 2014, Annual review of psychology.

[10]  B. B. Brown,et al.  Adolescents' Relationships with Peers , 2013 .

[11]  F. Van Overwalle Social cognition and the brain: A meta‐analysis , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[12]  E. Miller,et al.  Response to Comment on "Top-Down Versus Bottom-Up Control of Attention in the Prefrontal and Posterior Parietal Cortices" , 2007, Science.

[13]  Thomas J. Ross,et al.  Neuroanatomical dissociation between bottom–up and top–down processes of visuospatial selective attention , 2006, NeuroImage.

[14]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  R. Mar The neural bases of social cognition and story comprehension. , 2011, Annual review of psychology.

[16]  T. Q. Irigaray,et al.  Intellectual abilities in Alzheimer's disease patients: Contributions from the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) , 2010 .

[17]  Iroise Dumontheil,et al.  Social cognition and abstract thought in adolescence: the role of structural and functional development in rostral prefrontal cortex , 2012 .

[18]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Frontal and Temporo-Parietal Lobe Contributions to Theory of Mind: Neuropsychological Evidence from a False-Belief Task with Reduced Language and Executive Demands , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  Iroise Dumontheil,et al.  Development of relational reasoning during adolescence. , 2010, Developmental science.

[20]  Iroise Dumontheil,et al.  Taking perspective into account in a communicative task , 2010, NeuroImage.

[21]  Ian A. Apperly,et al.  Two sources of evidence on the non-automaticity of true and false belief ascription , 2010, Cognition.

[22]  Joaquín M. Fuster,et al.  Executive frontal functions , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  C. Frith,et al.  Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[24]  T. Ohnishi,et al.  Changes of brain activity in the neural substrates for theory of mind during childhood and adolescence , 2007, Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[25]  C. Frith,et al.  Adolescent development of the neural circuitry for thinking about intentions , 2007, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[26]  Iroise Dumontheil,et al.  Online usage of theory of mind continues to develop in late adolescence. , 2010, Developmental science.

[27]  Eirini Mavritsaki,et al.  Using biologically plausible neural models to specify the functional and neural mechanisms of visual search. , 2009, Progress in brain research.

[28]  Kathrin Cohen Kadosh,et al.  The social brain in adolescence: Evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging and behavioural studies , 2011, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[29]  Dana Samson,et al.  Seeing it my way: a case of a selective deficit in inhibiting self-perspective. , 2005, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[30]  Adam Qureshi,et al.  Why are there limits on theory of mind use? Evidence from adults’ ability to follow instructions from an ignorant speaker , 2010, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[31]  C. Frith,et al.  Social Cognition in Humans , 2007, Current Biology.

[32]  Frank Tong,et al.  Cognitive neuroscience: Primary visual cortex and visual awareness , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[33]  S. Blakemore,et al.  The role of puberty in the developing adolescent brain , 2010, Human brain mapping.

[34]  R. Knight,et al.  Frontal Lobe Contributions to Theory of Mind , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[35]  Iroise Dumontheil,et al.  Function and localization within rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[36]  I. Apperly Mindreaders: The Cognitive Basis of "Theory of Mind" , 2010 .

[37]  S. Carey,et al.  Understanding other minds: linking developmental psychology and functional neuroimaging. , 2004, Annual review of psychology.

[38]  A. Endress,et al.  The Social Sense: Susceptibility to Others’ Beliefs in Human Infants and Adults , 2010, Science.

[39]  Beate Sodian,et al.  Functional activity and effective connectivity of the posterior medial prefrontal cortex during processing of incongruent mental states , 2014, Human brain mapping.

[40]  Thomas F. Nugent,et al.  Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[41]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Level 1 perspective-taking at 24 months of age , 2006 .

[42]  J. Haxby,et al.  The distributed human neural system for face perception , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[43]  Mirella Dapretto,et al.  Developmental changes in the neural basis of interpreting communicative intent. , 2006, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[44]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Neural correlates of anchoring-and-adjustment during mentalizing , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[45]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  I Know You Are But What Am I?!: Neural Bases of Self- and Social Knowledge Retrieval in Children and Adults , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[46]  M Corbetta,et al.  Frontoparietal cortical networks for directing attention and the eye to visual locations: identical, independent, or overlapping neural systems? , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[47]  Susan Whitfield-Gabrieli,et al.  Brain regions for perceiving and reasoning about other people in school-aged children. , 2009, Child development.

[48]  Ivan Toni,et al.  Neural Correlates of Intentional Communication , 2010, Front. Neurosci..

[49]  D. Barr,et al.  Taking Perspective in Conversation: The Role of Mutual Knowledge in Comprehension , 2000, Psychological science.

[50]  John O. Willis,et al.  Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence , 2014 .

[51]  John Palmer,et al.  Visual search and attention: an overview. , 2004, Spatial vision.

[52]  Rebecca Saxe,et al.  Reading minds versus following rules: Dissociating theory of mind and executive control in the brain , 2006, Social neuroscience.

[53]  Eveline A. Crone,et al.  Executive functions in adolescence: inferences from brain and behavior. , 2009, Developmental science.

[54]  C. Frith,et al.  Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[55]  Dan Sperber,et al.  Pragmatics, modularity and mindreading , 2002 .

[56]  A. Leslie Developmental parallels in understanding minds and bodies , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[57]  Iroise Dumontheil,et al.  Dynamic causal modelling of effective connectivity during perspective taking in a communicative task , 2013, NeuroImage.

[58]  S. Bunge,et al.  Neurodevelopmental changes in working memory and cognitive control , 2007, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[59]  Geoffrey Bird,et al.  Development during Adolescence of the Neural Processing of Social Emotion , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[60]  Diane M. Beck,et al.  Top-down and bottom-up mechanisms in biasing competition in the human brain , 2009, Vision Research.

[61]  Frank Van Overwalle,et al.  A dissociation between social mentalizing and general reasoning , 2011, NeuroImage.

[62]  S. Blakemore,et al.  Development of the Selection and Manipulation of Self-Generated Thoughts in Adolescence , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[63]  James R. Booth,et al.  Neural development of selective attention and response inhibition , 2003, NeuroImage.

[64]  Gaia Scerif,et al.  Using developmental cognitive neuroscience to study behavioral and attentional control. , 2009, Developmental psychobiology.

[65]  Sarah-Jayne Blakemore,et al.  Neural processing associated with cognitive and affective Theory of Mind in adolescents and adults. , 2012, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[66]  R. Saxe Uniquely human social cognition , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[67]  Lindsey J. Powell,et al.  It's the Thought That Counts , 2006, Psychological science.

[68]  A. Toga,et al.  In vivo evidence for post-adolescent brain maturation in frontal and striatal regions , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[69]  S. Blakemore The social brain in adolescence , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[70]  Tracy L. Spinrad,et al.  Moral Cognitions and Prosocial Responding in Adolescence , 2009 .

[71]  J. Flavell,et al.  Young children's knowledge about visual perception: Further evidence for the Level 1–Level 2 distinction. , 1981 .

[72]  Asif A Ghazanfar,et al.  Different neural frequency bands integrate faces and voices differently in the superior temporal sulcus. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[73]  Carrie L. Masten,et al.  Neural correlates of direct and reflected self-appraisals in adolescents and adults: when social perspective-taking informs self-perception. , 2009, Child development.

[74]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The developing brain: From theory to neuroimaging and back , 2011, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[75]  C. Frith,et al.  Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’ , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[76]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Mechanical, behavioural and Intentional understanding of picture stories in autistic children , 1986 .

[77]  Hanneke E. M. den Ouden,et al.  Thinking about intentions , 2005, NeuroImage.

[78]  Sarah-Jayne Blakemore,et al.  Functional connectivity during a social emotion task in adolescents and in adults , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.

[79]  B Giesbrecht,et al.  Neural mechanisms of top-down control during spatial and feature attention , 2003, NeuroImage.

[80]  C. Summerfield,et al.  An information theoretical approach to prefrontal executive function , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[81]  Adriana Galvan,et al.  The adolescent brain. , 2008, Developmental review : DR.

[82]  J. Braithwaite,et al.  Seeing it their way: evidence for rapid and involuntary computation of what other people see. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[83]  Alison J. Wiggett,et al.  Functional MRI analysis of body and body part representations in the extrastriate and fusiform body areas. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[84]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Analysis of fMRI Time-Series Revisited , 1995, NeuroImage.