Decisions, Decisions: The Neurobiology of the effects of Dopamine Replacement Therapy on Decision-Making in Parkinson’s Disease

Dopamine replacement therapy (DRT) alleviates motor symptoms in Parkinson’s disease but induces neuropsychiatric side-effects. This review evaluates recent research into the decision-making deficits caused by DRT arising because dopamine ‘overdoses’ a relatively-intact ventral striatum while replenishing the dorsal striatum. Consequently, patients on medication are worse at learning from losses but better at learning from wins than healthy controls. Additionally, due to greater disruption of medication on limbic than cognitive neural circuits, patients are poorer at decision-making under risk than decision-making under ambiguity. Particularly, task components related to ventral fronto-striatal and orbitofrontal regions are affected more than those related to dorsal and prefrontal regions. Selective deficits in feedback processing and outcome evaluation due to limbic overdose likely drive this effect.

[1]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Modulation of memory fields by dopamine Dl receptors in prefrontal cortex , 1995, Nature.

[2]  U. Bonuccelli,et al.  Iowa Gambling Task in Parkinson's Disease , 2011, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[3]  Michael P. Milham,et al.  Distinct neural mechanisms of risk and ambiguity: A meta-analysis of decision-making , 2006, NeuroImage.

[4]  H O Fekjaer,et al.  [Pathological gambling]. , 2001, Tidsskrift for den Norske laegeforening : tidsskrift for praktisk medicin, ny raekke.

[5]  Colin Camerer,et al.  A framework for studying the neurobiology of value-based decision making , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[6]  A. Bechara,et al.  Decisions under ambiguity and decisions under risk: Correlations with executive functions and comparisons of two different gambling tasks with implicit and explicit rules , 2007, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[7]  Arnaud D'Argembeau,et al.  The Iowa Gambling Task in fMRI images , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[8]  S. Wylie,et al.  Trends and Issues in Characterizing Early Cognitive Changes in Parkinson’s Disease , 2012, Current Neurology and Neuroscience Reports.

[9]  E. Tolosa,et al.  Neuroanatomical correlates of impaired decision‐making and facial emotion recognition in early Parkinson’s disease , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.

[10]  C. Marsden,et al.  'Frontal' cognitive function in patients with Parkinson's disease 'on' and 'off' levodopa. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  Thomas V. Wiecki,et al.  Neurocomputational models of motor and cognitive deficits in Parkinson's disease. , 2010, Progress in brain research.

[12]  A. Cooper,et al.  Predictive Reward Signal of Dopamine Neurons , 2011 .

[13]  B. Dubois,et al.  Motivation, reward, and Parkinson’s disease: influence of dopatherapy , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  P. Dayan,et al.  Dopamine and performance in a reinforcement learning task: evidence from Parkinson's disease. , 2012 .

[15]  G. Fink,et al.  Decision-making under risk is improved by both dopaminergic medication and subthalamic stimulation in Parkinson's disease , 2014, Experimental Neurology.

[16]  William A. Cunningham,et al.  Executive Function: Mechanisms Underlying Emotion Regulation. , 2007 .

[17]  J. Borod,et al.  A Review of the Cognitive and Behavioral Sequelae of Parkinson's Disease: Relationship to Frontostriatal Circuitry , 2003, Cognitive and behavioral neurology : official journal of the Society for Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology.

[18]  Facundo Manes,et al.  Decision-making cognition in neurodegenerative diseases , 2010, Nature Reviews Neurology.

[19]  G. Deuschl,et al.  Patients with Parkinson's disease learn to control complex systems—an indication for intact implicit cognitive skill learning , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  M. Jahanshahi,et al.  Patients with Parkinson's disease learn to control complex systems via procedural as well as non-procedural learning , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  Donald P. Ely,et al.  Trends and Issues. , 1992 .

[22]  W. Jost,et al.  Drug-induced impulse control disorders in Parkinson’s disease , 2011, Journal of Neurology.

[23]  M. Raja,et al.  Impulsive and compulsive behaviors during dopamine replacement treatment in Parkinson's Disease and other disorders. , 2012, Current drug safety.

[24]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  By Carrot or by Stick: Cognitive Reinforcement Learning in Parkinsonism , 2004, Science.

[25]  A. Damasio,et al.  Deciding Advantageously Before Knowing the Advantageous Strategy , 1997, Science.

[26]  Peter Dayan,et al.  A Neural Substrate of Prediction and Reward , 1997, Science.

[27]  M Schulzer,et al.  Expectation and Dopamine Release: Mechanism of the Placebo Effect in Parkinson's Disease , 2001, Science.

[28]  Matthias Brand,et al.  Neuropsychological correlates of decision-making in ambiguous and risky situations , 2006, Neural Networks.

[29]  M. Hallett,et al.  Mechanisms Underlying Dopamine-Mediated Reward Bias in Compulsive Behaviors , 2010, Neuron.

[30]  A. Strafella,et al.  Dopamine Agonists Diminish Value Sensitivity of the Orbitofrontal Cortex: A Trigger for Pathological Gambling in Parkinson's Disease? , 2009, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[31]  Antonio P. Strafella,et al.  Impulsivity and Parkinson's disease: More than just disinhibition , 2011, Journal of the Neurological Sciences.

[32]  B. Sahakian,et al.  Deficits in decision-making in patients with aneurysms of the anterior communicating artery. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[33]  M. Hornberger,et al.  Beyond and below the cortex: the contribution of striatal dysfunction to cognition and behaviour in neurodegeneration , 2013, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry.

[34]  M. Gluck,et al.  Reward-learning and the novelty-seeking personality: a between- and within-subjects study of the effects of dopamine agonists on young Parkinson's patients. , 2009, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[35]  Erwan Bezard,et al.  Chronic dopaminergic stimulation in Parkinson's disease: from dyskinesias to impulse control disorders , 2009, The Lancet Neurology.

[36]  James J. Gross,et al.  Handbook of emotion regulation , 2007 .

[37]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Neural Systems Responding to Degrees of Uncertainty in Human Decision-Making , 2005, Science.

[38]  A. Lang,et al.  Impulse control disorders in Parkinson disease: a cross-sectional study of 3090 patients. , 2010, Archives of neurology.

[39]  T. Robbins,et al.  Enhanced or impaired cognitive function in Parkinson's disease as a function of dopaminergic medication and task demands. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[40]  A. Dagher,et al.  The role of dopamine in risk taking: a specific look at Parkinson’s disease and gambling , 2014, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[41]  M. Delazer,et al.  Decision making under risk and under ambiguity in Parkinson's disease , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[42]  T. Robbins,et al.  Differential effects of insular and ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions on risky decision-making , 2008, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[43]  M. Jahanshahi,et al.  What are people with Parkinson's disease really impaired on when it comes to making decisions? A meta-analysis of the evidence , 2013, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[44]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Prospect theory on the brain? Toward a cognitive neuroscience of decision under risk. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[45]  R. Hilker,et al.  Activation of basal ganglia loops in idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: a PET study , 2003, Journal of Neural Transmission.

[46]  M. Jahanshahi,et al.  The effects of dopaminergic medication on dynamic decision making in Parkinson's disease , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[47]  R. Cools Dopaminergic modulation of cognitive function-implications for l-DOPA treatment in Parkinson's disease , 2006, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[48]  F. Woermann,et al.  Decision Making under Risk Condition in Patients with Parkinson’s Disease: A Behavioural and fMRI Study , 2010, Behavioural neurology.

[49]  V. Voon,et al.  Medication-related impulse control and repetitive behaviors in Parkinson disease. , 2007, Archives of neurology.

[50]  U. Bonuccelli,et al.  Acute and chronic cognitive effects of levodopa and dopamine agonists on patients with Parkinson’s disease: a review , 2013, Therapeutic advances in psychopharmacology.

[51]  R. Dolan,et al.  Dopamine-dependent prediction errors underpin reward-seeking behaviour in humans , 2006, Nature.

[52]  N. Daw,et al.  Reinforcement Learning Signals in the Human Striatum Distinguish Learners from Nonlearners during Reward-Based Decision Making , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[53]  L. Timmermann,et al.  Dissociation of decision-making under ambiguity and decision-making under risk in patients with Parkinson's disease: A neuropsychological and psychophysiological study , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[54]  Penny A. MacDonald,et al.  Differential Effects of Dopaminergic Therapies on Dorsal and Ventral Striatum in Parkinson's Disease: Implications for Cognitive Function , 2011, Parkinson's Disease.

[55]  K Richard Ridderinkhof,et al.  The risky business of dopamine agonists in Parkinson disease and impulse control disorders. , 2011, Behavioral neuroscience.

[56]  D. Stein,et al.  Pathological gambling behaviour: Emergence secondary to treatment of Parkinson's disease with dopaminergic agents , 2000, Depression and anxiety.

[57]  Y. Geda,et al.  Pathological gambling caused by drugs used to treat Parkinson disease. , 2005, Archives of neurology.

[58]  Lorys Castelli,et al.  Reward pathways in Parkinson's disease: Clinical and theoretical implications , 2008, Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[59]  Daniel Weintraub,et al.  Dopamine and impulse control disorders in Parkinson's disease , 2008, Annals of neurology.

[60]  V. Voon,et al.  Medication-related impulse control and repetitive behaviors in Parkinson's disease , 2007, Current opinion in neurology.

[61]  O. Blin,et al.  Cognitive impulsivity in Parkinson's disease patients: Assessment and pathophysiology , 2009, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[62]  Roshan Cools,et al.  Greater striatal responses to medication in Parkinson׳s disease are associated with better task-switching but worse reward performance , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[63]  C. Padoa-Schioppa,et al.  Neurons in the orbitofrontal cortex encode economic value , 2006, Nature.

[64]  Rüdiger Hilker,et al.  Decision-Making Impairments in Patients with Parkinson’s Disease , 2005, Behavioural neurology.

[65]  T. Robbins,et al.  l-Dopa medication remediates cognitive inflexibility, but increases impulsivity in patients with Parkinson’s disease , 2003, Neuropsychologia.