Playing a puzzle video game with changing requirements improves executive functions

We trained college students to play one of four video games.Games ranged from action, puzzle, strategy and arcade type games.Only the puzzle game led to transfer on all executive function tasks. Recent research suggests a causal link between action video game playing and enhanced attention and visual-perceptual skills. In contrast, evidence linking action video games and enhanced executive function is equivocal. We investigated whether action and non-action video games enhance executive function. Fifty-five inexperienced video game players played one of four different games: an action video game (Modern Combat), a physics-based puzzle game (Cut the Rope), a real-time strategy game (Starfront Collision), and a fast paced arcade game (Fruit Ninja) for 20h. Three pre and post training tests of executive function were administered: a random task switching, a flanker, and a response inhibition task (Go/No-go). Only the group that trained on the physics-based puzzle game significantly improved in all three tasks relative to the pre-test. No training-related improvements were seen in other groups. These results suggest that playing a complex puzzle game that demands strategizing, reframing, and planning improves several aspects of executive function.

[1]  C. Carter,et al.  The anterior cingulate as a conflict monitor: fMRI and ERP studies , 2002, Physiology & Behavior.

[2]  R. Elliott,et al.  Response inhibition and impulsivity: an fMRI study , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task , 1997, Nature.

[4]  Rand R. Wilcox,et al.  New designs in analysis of variance. , 1987 .

[5]  John R. Anderson,et al.  The role of prefrontal cortex and posterior parietal cortex in task switching. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[6]  Andrew R. A. Conway,et al.  Working memory and retrieval: a resource-dependent inhibition model. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[7]  Gustavo Deco,et al.  Human Neuroscience Original Research Article Cortical Microcircuit Dynamics Mediating Binocular Rivalry: the Role of Adaptation in Inhibition , 2022 .

[8]  J. Unterrainer,et al.  When planning fails: individual differences and error-related brain activity in problem solving. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[9]  Walter Schneider,et al.  The cognitive control network: Integrated cortical regions with dissociable functions , 2007, NeuroImage.

[10]  P. Frensch,et al.  Effects of video game playing on measures of spatial performance: Gender effects in late adolescence. , 1994 .

[11]  R. Morris,et al.  Neural correlates of planning ability: Frontal lobe activation during the tower of London test , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  Brian E Maki,et al.  Attitudes of older adults toward shooter video games: An initial study to select an acceptable game for training visual processing. , 2010, Gerontechnology : international journal on the fundamental aspects of technology to serve the ageing society.

[13]  S. Monsell,et al.  Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .

[14]  B. Tabachnick,et al.  Using Multivariate Statistics , 1983 .

[15]  A. Miyake,et al.  The Nature and Organization of Individual Differences in Executive Functions , 2012, Current directions in psychological science.

[16]  C. Weiller,et al.  Dissociable contributions of left and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in planning. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Evidence for a two-stage model of spatial working memory processing within the lateral frontal cortex: a positron emission tomography study. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[18]  A. Dagher,et al.  Mapping the network for planning: a correlational PET activation study with the Tower of London task. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[19]  E. Thorndike,et al.  The influence of improvement in one mental function upon the efficiency of other functions. (I). , 1901 .

[20]  C S Green,et al.  Action-Video-Game Experience Alters the Spatial Resolution of Vision , 2007, Psychological science.

[21]  R. Engle,et al.  Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: the contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[22]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[23]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex , 1999, Nature.

[24]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled & automatic processing: behavior, theory, and biological mechanisms , 2003, Cogn. Sci..

[25]  Bruno Cozzi,et al.  Expression of calcium-binding proteins and selected neuropeptides in the human, chimpanzee, and crab-eating macaque claustrum , 2014, Front. Syst. Neurosci..

[26]  J R Anderson,et al.  Task preparation and task repetition: two-component model of task switching. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[27]  A. Cohen,et al.  Hierarchical systems of attention and action , 2005 .

[28]  T. Robbins,et al.  Planning and spatial working memory following frontal lobe lesions in man , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  Adrian M. Owen,et al.  Dopamine-Dependent Frontostriatal Planning Deficits in Early Parkinson's Disease , 1995 .

[30]  Jacob Cohen,et al.  A power primer. , 1992, Psychological bulletin.

[31]  Alan Kingstone,et al.  Improved top-down control reduces oculomotor capture: The case of action video game players , 2011, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[32]  Lars Bäckman,et al.  Transfer of Learning After Updating Training Mediated by the Striatum , 2008, Science.

[33]  R. Engle,et al.  The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: An individual-differences perspective , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[34]  Karen Murphy,et al.  Does Video Game Playing Improve Executive Functioning , 2006 .

[35]  C. S. Green,et al.  Action video game modifies visual selective attention , 2003, Nature.

[36]  H Pashler,et al.  Shifting visual attention and selecting motor responses: distinct attentional mechanisms. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[37]  A. Kramer,et al.  Can training in a real-time strategy video game attenuate cognitive decline in older adults? , 2008, Psychology and aging.

[38]  R. Mayer,et al.  Domain specificity of spatial expertise: The case of video game players. , 2002 .

[39]  J. Pekar,et al.  Meta-analysis of Go/No-go tasks demonstrating that fMRI activation associated with response inhibition is task-dependent , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  Julie I. Cohen,et al.  Effect of action video games on the spatial distribution of visuospatial attention. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[41]  Roger W. Remington,et al.  Not so fast: Rethinking the effects of action video games on attentional capacity , 2011 .

[42]  Matthew S Cain,et al.  Action video game experience reduces the cost of switching tasks , 2012, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[43]  Susan M. Ravizza,et al.  Shifting set about task switching: Behavioral and neural evidence for distinct forms of cognitive flexibility , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[44]  Maayan Pereg,et al.  Task switching training effects are mediated by working-memory management , 2013 .

[45]  Adam C. Oei,et al.  Are videogame training gains specific or general? , 2014, Front. Syst. Neurosci..

[46]  D. Bavelier,et al.  Neural bases of selective attention in action video game players , 2012, Vision Research.

[47]  K. Kiehl,et al.  Event‐related fMRI study of response inhibition , 2001, Human brain mapping.

[48]  U. Polat,et al.  Enhancing the contrast sensitivity function through action video game training , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[49]  Bernhard Hommel,et al.  DOOM'd to Switch: Superior Cognitive Flexibility in Players of First Person Shooter Games , 2010, Front. Psychology.

[50]  C. Anderson,et al.  Effects of Violent Video Games on Aggressive Behavior, Aggressive Cognition, Aggressive Affect, Physiological Arousal, and Prosocial Behavior: A Meta-Analytic Review of the Scientific Literature , 2001, Psychological science.

[51]  H. Garavan,et al.  Dissociable Executive Functions in the Dynamic Control of Behavior: Inhibition, Error Detection, and Correction , 2002, NeuroImage.

[52]  N. Lavie,et al.  On the Efficiency of Visual Selective Attention: Efficient Visual Search Leads to Inefficient Distractor Rejection , 1997 .

[53]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  Immature Frontal Lobe Contributions to Cognitive Control in Children Evidence from fMRI , 2002, Neuron.

[54]  Daphne Bavelier,et al.  The effect of action video game experience on task-switching , 2012, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[55]  J. Swets,et al.  A decision-making theory of visual detection. , 1954, Psychological review.

[56]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  Task-set reconfiguration with predictable and unpredictable task switches , 2003, Memory & cognition.

[57]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  Planning impairments in frontal lobe dementia and frontal lobe lesion patients , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[58]  Avishai Henik,et al.  Electroencephalographic Activity in a Flanker Interference Task Using Japanese Orthography , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[59]  Fabien Scalzo,et al.  Reducing backward masking through action game training. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[60]  Deana B. Davalos,et al.  Working memory, inhibition, and fluid intelligence as predictors of performance on Tower of Hanoi and London tasks , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[61]  M. Lezak,et al.  Neuropsychological assessment, 4th ed. , 2004 .

[62]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. , 1998, Science.

[63]  Á. Kristjánsson The case for causal influences of action videogame play upon vision and attention , 2013, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[64]  T. Shallice,et al.  Two networks involved in producing and realizing plans , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[65]  Joseph D. Chisholm,et al.  Reduced attentional capture in action video game players , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[66]  Adam C. Oei,et al.  Enhancing Cognition with Video Games: A Multiple Game Training Study , 2013, PloS one.

[67]  Randall W Engle,et al.  Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: a latent-variable approach. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[68]  James D. Johnson,et al.  The Influence of Media Violence on Youth , 2003, Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society.

[69]  M. J. Emerson,et al.  The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[70]  Steven A. Hillyard,et al.  Neural Basis of Superior Performance of Action Videogame Players in an Attention-Demanding Task , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[71]  R. Knight,et al.  Prefrontal–cingulate interactions in action monitoring , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[72]  Umberto Castiello,et al.  A neuroimaging study of selection-for-action: A reach-to-grasp study , 2004 .

[73]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and Automatic Human Information Processing: 1. Detection, Search, and Attention. , 1977 .

[74]  Colin M. Macleod The concept of inhibition in cognition. , 2007 .

[75]  Walter R. Boot,et al.  The Pervasive Problem With Placebos in Psychology , 2013, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[76]  Steven C. Dakin,et al.  Horizontal Information Drives the Behavioral Signatures of Face Processing , 2010, Front. Psychology.

[77]  J. Fuster Prefrontal Cortex , 2018 .

[78]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  A Developmental Functional MRI Study of Prefrontal Activation during Performance of a Go-No-Go Task , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[79]  A. Miyake,et al.  The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: a latent-variable analysis. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[80]  C. S. Green,et al.  Learning, Attentional Control, and Action Video Games , 2012, Current Biology.

[81]  Ulrich Mayr,et al.  On the role of verbalization during task set selection: Switching or serial order control? , 2005, Memory & cognition.

[82]  C. Pearce,et al.  Author's Note: Special Thanks for Their Support to Emily Morganti and Heather Logas of Telltale Games; Games and Culture Volume Xx Number X Month Xxxx Xx-xx the Truth about Baby Boomer Gamers a Study of Over-forty Computer Game Players , 2022 .

[83]  Torsten Schubert,et al.  Video game practice optimizes executive control skills in dual-task and task switching situations. , 2012, Acta psychologica.

[84]  D. Simons,et al.  The effects of video game playing on attention, memory, and executive control. , 2008, Acta psychologica.

[85]  D. Kort,et al.  Senior gamers : preferences, motivations and needs , 2009 .

[86]  T. Braver,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of frequency, inhibition and errors. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[87]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. , 1977 .

[88]  A Baddeley,et al.  Working memory and the control of action: evidence from task switching. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[89]  R. Engle,et al.  Working-memory capacity, proactive interference, and divided attention: limits on long-term memory retrieval. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[90]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Selective attention and response control following damage to the human pulvinar. , 2005 .

[91]  B. Tabachnick,et al.  Using multivariate statistics, 5th ed. , 2007 .

[92]  Roger W. Remington,et al.  Chronometric Evidence for two Types of Attention , 1995 .

[93]  C. S. Green,et al.  Enumeration versus multiple object tracking: the case of action video game players , 2006, Cognition.

[94]  A. Lutz,et al.  Mental Training as a Tool in the Neuroscientific Study of Brain and Cognitive Plasticity , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[95]  B. Kopp,et al.  N200 in the flanker task as a neurobehavioral tool for investigating executive control. , 1996, Psychophysiology.

[96]  E. Crone,et al.  Dissociation of response conflict, attentional selection, and expectancy with functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[97]  T. Shallice Specific impairments of planning. , 1982, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[98]  R. Barkley Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. , 1997, Psychological bulletin.

[99]  Hilde Haider,et al.  Preparatory processes in the task-switching paradigm: evidence from the use of probability cues. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[100]  Michael F. Bunting,et al.  The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: The importance of working memory capacity , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[101]  Sarena E. Saunders,et al.  Mapping the Network , 2008 .

[102]  Walter R. Boot,et al.  Video Games as a Means to Reduce Age-Related Cognitive Decline: Attitudes, Compliance, and Effectiveness , 2013, Front. Psychology.

[103]  P. Silvia,et al.  For Whom the Mind Wanders, and When , 2007, Psychological science.

[104]  B. Hommel,et al.  Action video gaming and cognitive control: playing first person shooter games is associated with improvement in working memory but not action inhibition , 2012, Psychological Research.

[105]  A. Dove,et al.  Prefrontal cortex activation in task switching: an event-related fMRI study. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[106]  A. Miyake,et al.  Not All Executive Functions Are Related to Intelligence , 2006, Psychological science.

[107]  J. Cockburn,et al.  Performance on the Tower of London test after severe head injury , 1995, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.