Face-specific and domain-general characteristics of cortical responses during self-recognition

The ability of visual self-recognition in animals and infants is considered a hallmark of the domain-general cognitive representation of the self, which also underpins higher social ability. Cortical regions activated during self-face recognition in human adults have been accordingly expected to play the domain-general role in self-processing. However, there is no evidence of the involvement of this network in non-face domains. We compared cortical responses during face and name recognition of self, a friend, and an unfamiliar person, using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Recognition of the self-face activated the right inferior frontal, precentral, supramarginal, and bilateral ventral occipitotemporal regions, consistent with previous findings, whereas these regions did not show self-specific activation during name recognition. During both face and name recognitions, increased activation for the friend and unfamiliar person than for the self was observed in the bilateral temporoparietal regions, and higher activation for the self and friend than for the unfamiliar person was observed in the medial cortical structures. These results suggest that the role of the self-specific networks during face recognition is not domain-general, but rather face-specific, and that the medial cortical structures, which are also implicated in self-referential processes, are not relevant to self-other distinction during face or name recognition. Instead, the reduced temporoparietal activation is a domain-general characteristic of the cortical response during self-recognition, which may reflect suppression of an automatic preparatory process for social interaction, possibly paralleling the disappearance of social behavior to the mirrored self-image at the emergence of self-recognition in animals and infants.

[1]  Hiroshi Fukuda,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of person representation: Recognition of famous and personally familiar names , 2006, NeuroImage.

[2]  Diana Reiss,et al.  Self-recognition in an Asian elephant , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[3]  M. Erb,et al.  Observing one’s hand become anarchic: An fMRI study of action identification , 2003, Consciousness and Cognition.

[4]  G. Fink,et al.  Cerebral Representation of One’s Own Past: Neural Networks Involved in Autobiographical Memory , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  D. Reiss,et al.  Mirror self-recognition in the bottlenose dolphin: A case of cognitive convergence , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[6]  Istvan Molnar-Szakacs,et al.  Self-face recognition activates a frontoparietal “mirror” network in the right hemisphere: an event-related fMRI study , 2005, NeuroImage.

[7]  Yasuhiro Maeda,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of visual self-recognition , 2005, NeuroImage.

[8]  C. N. Macrae,et al.  Mike or me? Self-recognition in a split-brain patient , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[9]  J. Binder,et al.  A Parametric Manipulation of Factors Affecting Task-induced Deactivation in Functional Neuroimaging , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[10]  K. Zilles,et al.  The neural correlates of person familiarity. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study with clinical implications. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  J. Haxby,et al.  Neural systems for recognition of familiar faces , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  G. Gallup Chimpanzees: Self-Recognition , 1970, Science.

[13]  J. Anderson,et al.  The development of self-recognition: a review. , 1984, Developmental psychobiology.

[14]  G. G. Gallup,et al.  Self‐awareness and the emergence of mind in primates , 1982, American journal of primatology.

[15]  Jessica A. Sommerville,et al.  Shared representations between self and other: a social cognitive neuroscience view , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[16]  Alvaro Pascual-Leone,et al.  Hand response differences in a self-face identification task , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  R Saxe,et al.  People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind” , 2003, NeuroImage.

[18]  C. Degueldre,et al.  Here I am: The cortical correlates of visual self-recognition , 2007, Brain Research.

[19]  R. Frackowiak,et al.  The neural systems sustaining face and proper-name processing , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[20]  G. Fink,et al.  Neural correlates of the first-person-perspective , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[21]  Peter J. B. Hancock,et al.  From Pixels to People: A Model of Familiar Face Recognition , 1999 .

[22]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[23]  J. Maunsell,et al.  Touching a Rubber Hand: Feeling of Body Ownership Is Associated with Activity in Multisensory Brain Areas , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  C. Frith,et al.  Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[25]  R. Kawashima,et al.  Passive and Active Recognition of One's Own Face , 2000, NeuroImage.

[26]  Maurizio Corbetta,et al.  Is the extrastriate body area involved in motor actions? , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[27]  P. Benson,et al.  Recognizing one's own face , 2001, Cognition.

[28]  T. Allison,et al.  Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[29]  L. Marino Convergence of Complex Cognitive Abilities in Cetaceans and Primates , 2002, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.

[30]  T. Feinberg,et al.  Where in the brain is the self? , 2005, Consciousness and Cognition.

[31]  Janet Treasure,et al.  I’m not as slim as that girl: Neural bases of body shape self-comparison to media images , 2007, NeuroImage.

[32]  Kosha Ruparel,et al.  Neural substrates for functionally discriminating self‐face from personally familiar faces , 2006, Human brain mapping.

[33]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Neurology: Self-recognition and the right hemisphere , 2001, Nature.

[34]  Uta Frith,et al.  Theory of mind , 2001, Current Biology.

[35]  Michael Lewis,et al.  Brain activation when hearing one's own and others' names , 2006, Brain Research.

[36]  Steven Laureys,et al.  Neural mechanisms involved in the detection of our first name: a combined ERPs and PET study , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[37]  B. Preilowski,et al.  Self-recognition as a test of consciousness in left and right hemisphere of "split-brain" patients. , 1977, Activitas nervosa superior.

[38]  Seth J. Gillihan,et al.  Is self special? A critical review of evidence from experimental psychology and cognitive neuroscience. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.

[39]  G. Gallup,et al.  Where am I? The neurological correlates of self and other. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[40]  Ellen Leibenluft,et al.  Social and emotional attachment in the neural representation of faces , 2004, NeuroImage.

[41]  J. Marshall,et al.  The neural consequences of conflict between intention and the senses. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[42]  C. Frith,et al.  “Hey John”: Signals Conveying Communicative Intention toward the Self Activate Brain Regions Associated with “Mentalizing,” Regardless of Modality , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  P. Benson,et al.  Towards a functional neuroanatomy of self processing: effects of faces and words. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[44]  G. Shulman,et al.  Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain function , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[45]  J. Haxby,et al.  The distributed human neural system for face perception , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[46]  Tokiko Harada,et al.  The role of the right prefrontal cortex in self-evaluation of the face: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study , 2008 .

[47]  M. Iacoboni,et al.  The self and social cognition: the role of cortical midline structures and mirror neurons , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[48]  Steven M Platek,et al.  Cross-modal self-recognition: The role of visual, auditory, and olfactory primes , 2004, Consciousness and Cognition.

[49]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Self-recognition and the right prefrontal cortex , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[50]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[51]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Extrastriate body area in human occipital cortex responds to the performance of motor actions , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[52]  Shigeru Sato,et al.  Multiple brain networks for visual self-recognition with different sensitivity for motion and body part , 2006, NeuroImage.

[53]  N. Brady,et al.  My left brain and me: a dissociation in the perception of self and others , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[54]  R. Buckner,et al.  Self-projection and the brain , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[55]  Andrew J. Calder,et al.  Self Priming: A Short term Benefit of Repetition , 1996 .

[56]  A. Nakamura,et al.  Neural substrates for recognition of familiar voices: a PET study , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[57]  J. Shoshani,et al.  Elephant brain Part I: Gross morphology, functions, comparative anatomy, and evolution , 2006, Brain Research Bulletin.