Generous or Parsimonious Cognitive Architecture? Cognitive Neuroscience and Theory of Mind

Recent work in cognitive neuroscience on the child's Theory of Mind (ToM) has pursued the idea that the ability to metarepresent mental states depends on a domain-specific cognitive subystem implemented in specific neural circuitry: a Theory of Mind Module. We argue that the interaction of several domain-general mechanisms and lower-level domain-specific mechanisms accounts for the flexibility and sophistication of behavior, which has been taken to be evidence for a domain-specific ToM module. This finding is of more general interest since it suggests a parsimonious cognitive architecture can account for apparent domain specificity. We argue for such an architecture in two stages. First, on conceptual grounds, contrasting the case of language with ToM, and second, by showing that recent evidence in the form of fMRI and lesion studies supports the more parsimonious hypothesis. 1. Theory of Mind, Metarepresentation, and Modularity2. Developmental Components of ToM3. The Analogy with Modularity of Language4. Dissociations without Modules5. The Evidence from Neuroscience6. Conclusion Theory of Mind, Metarepresentation, and Modularity Developmental Components of ToM The Analogy with Modularity of Language Dissociations without Modules The Evidence from Neuroscience Conclusion

[1]  D. Neary,et al.  Social cognition in frontotemporal dementia and Huntington’s disease , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  V. Stone Theory of mind and the evolution of social intelligence , 2005 .

[3]  M. D. Rutherford,et al.  The Perception of Animacy in Young Children with Autism , 2006, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[4]  R Saxe,et al.  People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind” , 2003, NeuroImage.

[5]  Bruce F. Pennington,et al.  Understanding of Others' Intentions in Children with Autism , 2001, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[6]  A. Whiten,et al.  Mental evolution and development: Evidence for secondary representation in children, great apes, and other animals. , 2001 .

[7]  A. Leslie Pretending and believing: issues in the theory of ToMM , 1994, Cognition.

[8]  D. Zaitchik,et al.  When representations conflict with reality: The preschooler's problem with false beliefs and “false” photographs , 1990, Cognition.

[9]  G. Kanjilal Autistic Children , 1971, Mind and Mental Health Magazine.

[10]  H. Wellman,et al.  Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development , 2001 .

[11]  U. Frith,et al.  The impact of extensive medial frontal lobe damage on 'Theory of Mind' and cognition. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[12]  Ianthi Maria Tsimpli,et al.  The mind of a savant: Language learning and modularity. , 1997 .

[13]  Noam Chomsky Knowledge of Language , 1986 .

[14]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  The detection of contingency and animacy from simple animations in the human brain. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[15]  B. Pennington,et al.  Executive functions in young children with autism. , 1999, Child development.

[16]  R. Schultz Developmental deficits in social perception in autism: the role of the amygdala and fusiform face area , 2005, International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience.

[17]  A. J. Mistlin,et al.  Social signals analyzed at the single cell level : someone is looking at me, something touched me, something moved! , 1990 .

[18]  L. Moses,et al.  Individual differences in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind. , 2001, Child development.

[19]  D. Povinelli Theories of theories of mind: Chimpanzee theory of mind?: the long road to strong inference , 1996 .

[20]  H. V. D. Lely,et al.  Binding theory and grammatical specific language impairment in children , 1997, Cognition.

[21]  F. Volkmar,et al.  A Normed Study of Face Recognition in Autism and Related Disorders , 1999, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[22]  Dana Samson,et al.  Left temporoparietal junction is necessary for representing someone else's belief , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  S. Carey,et al.  Understanding other minds: linking developmental psychology and functional neuroimaging. , 2004, Annual review of psychology.

[24]  J. Fodor The Modularity of mind. An essay on faculty psychology , 1986 .

[25]  神代 真里 Bidirectional communication between a Japanese monkey and a human through eye gaze and pointing , 2004 .

[26]  D. Povinelli,et al.  Chimpanzee minds: suspiciously human? , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[27]  X. Zhe What is "Children's Theory of Mind"? , 2001 .

[28]  Thomas R. Keenan Memory Span as a Predictor of False Belief Understanding , 1998 .

[29]  L. Cosmides,et al.  Cognitive adaptations for social exchange. , 1992 .

[30]  Alan M. Leslie,et al.  Domain specificity in conceptual development: Neuropsychological evidence from autism , 1992, Cognition.

[31]  J. Elman,et al.  Rethinking Innateness: A Connectionist Perspective on Development , 1996 .

[32]  J. Haxby,et al.  Distinct representations of eye gaze and identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[33]  R. Knight,et al.  Frontal Lobe Contributions to Theory of Mind , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[34]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not see , 2000, Animal Behaviour.

[35]  Kim Sterelny,et al.  From mating to mentality: evaluating evolutionary psychology , 2003 .

[36]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind” ? , 1985, Cognition.

[37]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  A mathematician, a physicist and a computer scientist with Asperger syndrome: Performance on folk psychology and folk physics tests , 1999 .

[38]  A. Leslie Pretense and representation: The origins of "theory of mind." , 1987 .

[39]  György Gergely,et al.  One-year-old infants use teleological representations of actions productively , 2003, Cogn. Sci..

[40]  G. Dawson,et al.  Early recognition of children with autism: A study of first birthday home videotapes , 1994, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[41]  K. Sterelny The Representational Theory Of Mind , 1991 .

[42]  Kim Sterelny,et al.  Sex and Death. An Introduction to Philosophy of Biology (M. Matthen) , 1999 .

[43]  Thomas Suddendorf,et al.  The Rise of the Metamind , 1997 .

[44]  A. Karmiloff-Smith,et al.  Double Dissociations in Developmental Disorders? Theoretically Misconceived, Empirically Dubious , 2003, Cortex.

[45]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Testing the domain-specificity of a theory of mind deficit in brain-injured patients: Evidence for consistent performance on non-verbal, “reality-unknown” false belief and false photograph tasks , 2007, Cognition.

[46]  J. Hodges,et al.  Theory of mind in frontotemporal dementia , 2006, Social neuroscience.

[47]  A. Karmiloff-Smith Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[48]  D. Perrett,et al.  A differential neural response in the human amygdala to fearful and happy facial expressions , 1996, Nature.

[49]  S. Ozonoff Reliability and validity of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in studies of autism. , 1995 .

[50]  M. D. Rutherford,et al.  Cognitive Underpinnings of Pretend Play in Autism , 2003, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[51]  O. I. Lovaas,et al.  The autistic child , 1977 .

[52]  Ian Apperly,et al.  False belief reasoning and the acquisition of relative clause sentences. , 2003, Child development.

[53]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension , 1995, Cognition.

[54]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Frontal and Temporo-Parietal Lobe Contributions to Theory of Mind: Neuropsychological Evidence from a False-Belief Task with Reduced Language and Executive Demands , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[55]  Jennie E. Pyers,et al.  Complements to cognition: a longitudinal study of the relationship between complex syntax and false-belief-understanding , 2002 .

[56]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Children with Autism Fail to Orient to Naturally Occurring Social Stimuli , 1998, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[57]  Geraldine Dawson,et al.  Young children with autism show atypical brain responses to fearful versus neutral facial expressions of emotion. , 2004, Developmental science.

[58]  Max Coltheart,et al.  Modularity and cognition , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[59]  Philip Gerrans The theory of mind module in evolutionary psychology , 2002 .

[60]  G. Baird,et al.  A screening instrument for autism at 18 months of age: a 6-year follow-up study. , 2000, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[61]  Animacy and propulsion in infancy: Tracking, waving and reaching to self‐propelled and induced moving objects , 1999 .

[62]  G. Baird,et al.  Testing joint attention, imitation, and play as infancy precursors to language and theory of mind , 2000 .

[63]  S. Baron-Cohen Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind , 1997 .

[64]  B. Pennington,et al.  Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: relationship to theory of mind. , 1991, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[65]  R. Samuels Evolutionary Psychology and the Massive Modularity Hypothesis , 1998, The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science.

[66]  K. Nakamura,et al.  The human amygdala plays an important role in gaze monitoring. A PET study. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[67]  Philip Gerrans Nativism and Neuroconstructivism in the Explanation of Williams Syndrome , 2003 .

[68]  L. Moses,et al.  Executive functioning and preschoolers' understanding of false beliefs, false photographs, and false signs. , 2006, Child development.

[69]  D. Perrett,et al.  Neural Representation for the Perception of the Intentionality of Actions , 2000, Brain and Cognition.

[70]  A. Woodward Infants' ability to distinguish between purposeful and non-purposeful behaviors , 1999 .

[71]  L. Cosmides,et al.  Beyond intuition and instinct blindness: toward an evolutionarily rigorous cognitive science , 1994, Cognition.

[72]  J. Boucher,et al.  Unfamiliar face recognition in relatively able autistic children. , 1992, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[73]  G. Dawson,et al.  Early social attention impairments in autism: social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. , 2004, Developmental psychology.

[74]  S. Gelman,et al.  Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity In Cognition And Culture , 1994 .

[75]  M. Wheeler,et al.  The Grain of Domains: The Evolutionary-Psychological Case Against Domain-General Cognition , 2004 .

[76]  H. Wellman,et al.  Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. , 2001, Child development.

[77]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Neural Correlates of Eye-Gaze Detection in Young Children with Autism , 2005, Cortex.

[78]  S. Pinker Learnability and Cognition: The Acquisition of Argument Structure , 1989 .

[79]  Richard J. Brown Neuropsychology Mental Structure , 1989 .

[80]  A. Cowey,et al.  Sensitivity to eye gaze in prosopagnosic patients and monkeys with superior temporal sulcus ablation , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[81]  A. Karmiloff-Smith,et al.  Is There a Social Module? Language, Face Processing, and Theory of Mind in Individuals with Williams Syndrome , 1995, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[82]  F. Happé,et al.  Acquired mind-blindness following frontal lobe surgery? A single case study of impaired ‘theory of mind’ in a patient treated with stereotactic anterior capsulotomy , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[83]  C. Frith,et al.  Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’ , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.