Nakayama, Shimojo, and Ramachandran's 1990 Paper

The target paper reviewed in this article was titled "Transparency: relation to depth, subjective contours, luminance, and neon colour spreading" coauthored by K Nakayama, S Shimojo, and V S Ramachandran, published in 1990. This paper, one of the first in a series on surface perception, examined how in untextured stereograms, local disparity and luminance contrast can drastically change surface quality, subjective contours, and the effect of neon colour spreading. When we began to conceive this and related work, the ascendant view on visual perception was derived from the pioneering studies of the response properties of visual neurons with microelectrodes, including those of Barlow (1953), Lettvin et al (1959), and Hubel and Wiesel (1959, 1962). All suggested that there are remarkable operations on the image by earliest stages of the visual pathway, which bestowed selectivity to colour, orientation, motion direction, spatial frequency, binocular disparity, etc. As such, it would seem that an understanding of vision would come through more systematic description of the properties of single neuron selectivities. This viewpoint was well summarised by Horace Barlow in his famous neuron doctrine paper (Barlow 1972), which emphasised the importance of analysing the image in successive stages of processing by neurons with specific classes of receptive fields. Later work altered this conception somewhat by seeing receptive fields as linear filters. Rather than detecting the presence of edges, bars, or otherwise perceptually identifiable elements in a scene, cells were seen as making measurements of an image. It was an 'image based' approach to vision, treating the basic operations of vision with no particular regard as to what aspects of scenes were being coded, whether a given cell's response corresponded to something about surfaces, edges, or objects in the real world.

[1]  F. Qiu,et al.  Figure and Ground in the Visual Cortex: V2 Combines Stereoscopic Cues with Gestalt Rules , 2005, Neuron.

[2]  Shinsuke Shimojo,et al.  Visual surface representation: a critical link between lower-level and higher level vision , 1995 .

[3]  Manish Singh,et al.  The perceived transmittance of inhomogeneous surfaces and media , 2006, Vision Research.

[4]  Maggie Shiffrar,et al.  The influence of terminators on motion integration across space , 1992, Vision Research.

[5]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex , 1962, The Journal of physiology.

[6]  B. Julesz,et al.  A THEORETICAL ANALYSIS OF ILLUSORY CONTOUR FORMATION IN STEREOPSIS , 1995 .

[7]  L. Spillmann,et al.  The Neon Color Effect in the Ehrenstein Illusion , 1981, Perception.

[8]  E. Mingolla,et al.  Neon Color Spreading: A Review , 1997, Perception.

[9]  Shinsuke Shimojo,et al.  Transparency: Relation to depth, subjective contours and color spreading , 1990 .

[10]  H. F. van Tuijl,et al.  Sensory Conditions for the Occurrence of the Neon Spreading Illusion , 1979, Perception.

[11]  P. Kellman,et al.  Interpolation processes in object perception: reply to Anderson (2007). , 2007, Psychological review.

[12]  Takeo Watanabe,et al.  The primary visual cortex fills in color , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[13]  F. Qiu,et al.  Neural representation of transparent overlay , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[14]  Ken Nakayama,et al.  Brightness perception and filling-in , 1991, Vision Research.

[15]  K Nakayama,et al.  Experiencing and perceiving visual surfaces. , 1992, Science.

[16]  Shinsuke Shimojo,et al.  Da vinci stereopsis: Depth and subjective occluding contours from unpaired image points , 1990, Vision Research.

[17]  L. Spillmann Phenomenology and neurophysiological correlations: Two approaches to perception research , 2009, Vision Research.

[18]  L. Isbell,et al.  Snakes as agents of evolutionary change in primate brains. , 2006, Journal of human evolution.

[19]  Manish Singh,et al.  Toward a perceptual theory of transparency. , 2002, Psychological review.

[20]  Barton L Anderson,et al.  The demise of the identity hypothesis and the insufficiency and nonnecessity of contour relatability in predicting object interpolation: comment on Kellman, Garrigan, and Shipley (2005). , 2007, Psychological review.

[21]  R O Duncan,et al.  Occlusion and the Interpretation of Visual Motion: Perceptual and Neuronal Effects of Context , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  D. C. Essen,et al.  Visual areas of the mammalian cerebral cortex. , 1979 .

[23]  R. Hetherington The Perception of the Visual World , 1952 .

[24]  H. Barlow Summation and inhibition in the frog's retina , 1953, The Journal of physiology.

[25]  Ken Nakayama,et al.  Resolving Border Disputes in Midlevel Vision , 2005, Neuron.

[26]  P U Tse,et al.  Amodal Completion in the Absence of Image Tangent Discontinuities , 1998, Perception.

[27]  S. Grossberg,et al.  Neural dynamics of form perception: boundary completion, illusory figures, and neon color spreading. , 1985 .

[28]  M. D'Zmura,et al.  Test of a Convergence Model for Color Transparency Perception , 1998, Perception.

[29]  Jonathan Winawer,et al.  Image segmentation and lightness perception , 2005, Nature.

[30]  Jitendra Malik,et al.  Computational framework for determining stereo correspondence from a set of linear spatial filters , 1992, Image Vis. Comput..

[31]  E H Adelson,et al.  Beyond Junctions: Nonlocal form Constraints on Motion Interpretation , 2001, Perception.

[32]  K Nakayama,et al.  Visual attention to surfaces in three-dimensional space. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[33]  B. Anderson The role of occlusion in the perception of depth, lightness, and opacity. , 2003, Psychological review.

[34]  D. Hubel,et al.  Segregation of form, color, movement, and depth: anatomy, physiology, and perception. , 1988, Science.

[35]  DH Hubel,et al.  Psychophysical evidence for separate channels for the perception of form, color, movement, and depth , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[36]  D Purves,et al.  Why we see things the way we do: evidence for a wholly empirical strategy of vision. , 2001, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[37]  Manish Singh,et al.  Perceptual Assignment of Opacity to Translucent Surfaces: The Role of Image Blur , 2002, Perception.

[38]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields of single neurones in the cat's striate cortex , 1959, The Journal of physiology.

[39]  C. Blakemore,et al.  The neural mechanism of binocular depth discrimination , 1967, The Journal of physiology.

[40]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Real world occlusion constraints and binocular rivalry , 1990, Vision Research.

[41]  H. F. van Tuijl A new visual illusion: neonlike color spreading and complementary color induction between subjective contours. , 1975, Acta psychologica.

[42]  Xin Huang,et al.  Lightness, filling-in, and the fundamental role of context in visual perception. , 2006, Progress in brain research.

[43]  Zijiang J. He,et al.  Surfaces versus features in visual search , 1992, Nature.

[44]  B. Anderson A Theory of Illusory Lightness and Transparency in Monocular and Binocular Images: The Role of Contour Junctions , 1997, Perception.

[45]  K Nakayama,et al.  Stereoscopic Depth: Its Relation to Image Segmentation, Grouping, and the Recognition of Occluded Objects , 1989, Perception.

[46]  R. von der Heydt,et al.  Coding of Border Ownership in Monkey Visual Cortex , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[47]  Zijiang J. He,et al.  Perceived surface shape not features determines correspondence strength in apparent motion , 1994, Vision Research.

[48]  G. Poggio,et al.  Binocular interaction and depth sensitivity in striate and prestriate cortex of behaving rhesus monkey. , 1977, Journal of neurophysiology.

[49]  S. Shimojo,et al.  Illusory Occluding Contours and Surface Formation by Depth Propagation , 1992, Perception.

[50]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  Transparency: Relation to Depth, Subjective Contours, Luminance, and Neon Color Spreading , 1990, Perception.

[51]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Perceptual filling-in: a parametric study , 1998, Vision Research.

[52]  R. von der Heydt,et al.  Illusory contours and cortical neuron responses. , 1984, Science.

[53]  Barton L Anderson,et al.  Stereoscopic Surface Perception , 1999, Neuron.

[54]  G. H. Jacobs THE DISTRIBUTION AND NATURE OF COLOUR VISION AMONG THE MAMMALS , 1993, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.

[55]  Manish Singh,et al.  Photometric determinants of perceived transparency , 2006, Vision Research.

[56]  J. Bakin,et al.  Visual Responses in Monkey Areas V1 and V2 to Three-Dimensional Surface Configurations , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[57]  L. Spillmann,et al.  Colored neon flanks and line gap enhancement , 1984, Vision Research.

[58]  Li Zhaoping,et al.  Border Ownership from Intracortical Interactions in Visual Area V2 , 2005, Neuron.

[59]  H. Barlow,et al.  Single Units and Sensation: A Neuron Doctrine for Perceptual Psychology? , 1972, Perception.

[60]  E. Brunswik The conceptual focus of some psychological systems , 1939 .

[61]  M K Albert,et al.  The Generic-Viewpoint Assumption and Illusory Contours , 2000, Perception.

[62]  F Metelli,et al.  The perception of transparency. , 1974, Scientific American.

[63]  H. Barlow Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information: David Marr. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman, 1982. pp. xvi + 397 , 1983 .

[64]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Occlusion and the solution to the aperture problem for motion , 1989, Vision Research.

[65]  David Marr,et al.  VISION A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information , 2009 .

[66]  Barton L. Anderson,et al.  The role of partial occlusion in stereopsis , 1994, Nature.

[67]  S. Grossberg,et al.  Neural dynamics of form perception: boundary completion, illusory figures, and neon color spreading. , 1985, Psychological review.

[68]  Shimon Ullman,et al.  Class-Specific, Top-Down Segmentation , 2002, ECCV.

[69]  B. Anderson Filling-in models of completion: rejoinder to Kellman, Garrigan, Shipley, and Keane (2007) and Albert (2007). , 2007, Psychological review.

[70]  Susumu Tachi,et al.  An integrative model of binocular vision: a stereo model utilizing interocularly unpaired points produces both depth and binocular rivalry , 2004, Vision Research.