Default network connectivity reflects the level of consciousness in non-communicative brain-damaged patients.

The 'default network' is defined as a set of areas, encompassing posterior-cingulate/precuneus, anterior cingulate/mesiofrontal cortex and temporo-parietal junctions, that show more activity at rest than during attention-demanding tasks. Recent studies have shown that it is possible to reliably identify this network in the absence of any task, by resting state functional magnetic resonance imaging connectivity analyses in healthy volunteers. However, the functional significance of these spontaneous brain activity fluctuations remains unclear. The aim of this study was to test if the integrity of this resting-state connectivity pattern in the default network would differ in different pathological alterations of consciousness. Fourteen non-communicative brain-damaged patients and 14 healthy controls participated in the study. Connectivity was investigated using probabilistic independent component analysis, and an automated template-matching component selection approach. Connectivity in all default network areas was found to be negatively correlated with the degree of clinical consciousness impairment, ranging from healthy controls and locked-in syndrome to minimally conscious, vegetative then coma patients. Furthermore, precuneus connectivity was found to be significantly stronger in minimally conscious patients as compared with unconscious patients. Locked-in syndrome patient's default network connectivity was not significantly different from controls. Our results show that default network connectivity is decreased in severely brain-damaged patients, in proportion to their degree of consciousness impairment. Future prospective studies in a larger patient population are needed in order to evaluate the prognostic value of the presented methodology.

[1]  Alexa M. Morcom,et al.  Does the brain have a baseline? Why we should be resisting a rest , 2007, NeuroImage.

[2]  M. Boly,et al.  Diagnostic accuracy of the vegetative and minimally conscious state: Clinical consensus versus standardized neurobehavioral assessment , 2009, BMC neurology.

[3]  Steven Laureys,et al.  The changing spectrum of coma , 2008, Nature Clinical Practice Neurology.

[4]  S. Rombouts,et al.  Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[5]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  Classification of fMRI independent components using IC-fingerprints and support vector machine classifiers , 2007, NeuroImage.

[6]  A. Braun,et al.  Decoupling of the brain's default mode network during deep sleep , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[7]  Steven Laureys,et al.  Brain function in coma, vegetative state, and related disorders , 2004, The Lancet Neurology.

[8]  M. Raichle The Brain's Dark Energy , 2006, Science.

[9]  J. Winn,et al.  Brain , 1878, The Lancet.

[10]  Dietmar Cordes,et al.  Comparison of independent component analysis and conventional hypothesis-driven analysis for clinical functional MR image processing. , 2002, AJNR. American journal of neuroradiology.

[11]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Intrinsic functional architecture in the anaesthetized monkey brain , 2007, Nature.

[12]  S Laureys,et al.  Intrinsic Brain Activity in Altered States of Consciousness , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[13]  J. Changeux,et al.  Ongoing Spontaneous Activity Controls Access to Consciousness: A Neuronal Model for Inattentional Blindness , 2005, PLoS biology.

[14]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  Investigations into resting-state connectivity using independent component analysis , 2005, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[15]  Steven Laureys,et al.  What is it like to be vegetative or minimally conscious? , 2007, Current opinion in neurology.

[16]  A. Cavanna,et al.  The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[17]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[18]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Generalisability, Random Effects & Population Inference , 1998, NeuroImage.

[19]  J. Born The Glasgow-Liège Scale. Prognostic value and evolution of motor response and brain stem reflexes after severe head injury. , 1988, Acta neurochirurgica.

[20]  A. Hijdra,et al.  PET scanning and neuronal loss in acute vegetative state , 2000, The Lancet.

[21]  J. Giacino,et al.  The minimally conscious state: Definition and diagnostic criteria , 2002, Neurology.

[22]  M. Boly,et al.  Functional connectivity in the default network during resting state is preserved in a vegetative but not in a brain dead patient , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[23]  J. Giacino The minimally conscious state: defining the borders of consciousness. , 2005, Progress in brain research.

[24]  M. Boly,et al.  Auditory processing in severely brain injured patients: differences between the minimally conscious state and the persistent vegetative state. , 2004, Archives of neurology.

[25]  P. Fransson Spontaneous low‐frequency BOLD signal fluctuations: An fMRI investigation of the resting‐state default mode of brain function hypothesis , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[26]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[27]  F. Plum,et al.  Behavioural improvements with thalamic stimulation after severe traumatic brain injury , 2008, Nature.

[28]  M. Raichle,et al.  Cortical network functional connectivity in the descent to sleep , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[29]  C. F. Beckmann,et al.  Tensorial extensions of independent component analysis for multisubject FMRI analysis , 2005, NeuroImage.

[30]  J. Born The Glasgow-Liège Scale , 2005, Acta Neurochirurgica.

[31]  V. Haughton,et al.  Frequencies contributing to functional connectivity in the cerebral cortex in "resting-state" data. , 2001, AJNR. American journal of neuroradiology.

[32]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  Probabilistic independent component analysis for functional magnetic resonance imaging , 2004, IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging.

[33]  A. Cavanna The Precuneus and Consciousness , 2007, CNS Spectrums.

[34]  M. Greicius,et al.  Resting-state functional connectivity reflects structural connectivity in the default mode network. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[35]  J. Giacino,et al.  The JFK Coma Recovery Scale-Revised: measurement characteristics and diagnostic utility. , 2004, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[36]  Steven Laureys,et al.  Self-consciousness in non-communicative patients , 2007, Consciousness and Cognition.

[37]  W. Liao,et al.  Impaired perceptual networks in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by resting fMRI , 2009, Journal of Neurology.

[38]  B. Biswal,et al.  The resting brain: unconstrained yet reliable. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[39]  Steve Majerus,et al.  Behavioral evaluation of consciousness in severe brain damage. , 2005, Progress in brain research.

[40]  M. Fukunaga,et al.  Low frequency BOLD fluctuations during resting wakefulness and light sleep: A simultaneous EEG‐fMRI study , 2008, Human brain mapping.

[41]  Steven Laureys The neural correlate of (un)awareness: lessons from the vegetative state , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[42]  Steven Laureys,et al.  Perception of pain in the minimally conscious state with PET activation: an observational study , 2008, The Lancet Neurology.

[43]  Peter Fransson,et al.  The precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex plays a pivotal role in the default mode network: Evidence from a partial correlation network analysis , 2008, NeuroImage.

[44]  D. Shewmon,et al.  The minimally conscious state: definition and diagnostic criteria. , 2002, Neurology.

[45]  S Makeig,et al.  Analysis of fMRI data by blind separation into independent spatial components , 1998, Human brain mapping.

[46]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Wandering Minds: The Default Network and Stimulus-Independent Thought , 2007, Science.

[47]  G. Glover,et al.  Dissociable Intrinsic Connectivity Networks for Salience Processing and Executive Control , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  fMRI resting state networks define distinct modes of long-distance interactions in the human brain , 2006, NeuroImage.

[49]  Nathan S White,et al.  Impaired thalamocortical connectivity in humans during general-anesthetic-induced unconsciousness , 2003, NeuroImage.

[50]  O. Sporns,et al.  Mapping the Structural Core of Human Cerebral Cortex , 2008, PLoS biology.

[51]  Marcus E Raichle,et al.  Neuroscience. The brain's dark energy. , 2006, Science.

[52]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: A primer with examples , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[53]  G. Glover,et al.  Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Major Depression: Abnormally Increased Contributions from Subgenual Cingulate Cortex and Thalamus , 2007, Biological Psychiatry.

[54]  M. Greicius,et al.  Persistent default‐mode network connectivity during light sedation , 2008, Human brain mapping.

[55]  M. Greicius,et al.  Default-mode network activity distinguishes Alzheimer's disease from healthy aging: Evidence from functional MRI , 2004, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA.

[56]  J. Pekar,et al.  Different activation dynamics in multiple neural systems during simulated driving , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[57]  B. Baars,et al.  Brain, conscious experience and the observing self , 2003, Trends in Neurosciences.