Simultaneous modeling of visual saliency and value computation improves predictions of economic choice

Significance Many everyday decisions require viewing displays with several alternatives and then rapidly choosing one, e.g., choosing a snack from a vending machine. Each item has objective visual properties, such as saliency, and subjective properties, such as value. Objective and subjective properties are usually studied independently. We have implemented a single integrated paradigm that links perceptual with economic decision making by having subjects search through visual displays to choose a single food item that they want to eat. We demonstrate that two linked accumulator models, one modeling perceptual and the other modeling economic decisions, account for subjects’ viewing patterns and choices. Many decisions we make require visually identifying and evaluating numerous alternatives quickly. These usually vary in reward, or value, and in low-level visual properties, such as saliency. Both saliency and value influence the final decision. In particular, saliency affects fixation locations and durations, which are predictive of choices. However, it is unknown how saliency propagates to the final decision. Moreover, the relative influence of saliency and value is unclear. Here we address these questions with an integrated model that combines a perceptual decision process about where and when to look with an economic decision process about what to choose. The perceptual decision process is modeled as a drift–diffusion model (DDM) process for each alternative. Using psychophysical data from a multiple-alternative, forced-choice task, in which subjects have to pick one food item from a crowded display via eye movements, we test four models where each DDM process is driven by (i) saliency or (ii) value alone or (iii) an additive or (iv) a multiplicative combination of both. We find that models including both saliency and value weighted in a one-third to two-thirds ratio (saliency-to-value) significantly outperform models based on either quantity alone. These eye fixation patterns modulate an economic decision process, also described as a DDM process driven by value. Our combined model quantitatively explains fixation patterns and choices with similar or better accuracy than previous models, suggesting that visual saliency has a smaller, but significant, influence than value and that saliency affects choices indirectly through perceptual decisions that modulate economic decisions.

[1]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Microstimulation of visual cortex affects the speed of perceptual decisions , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[2]  Ian Krajbich,et al.  Visual fixations and the computation and comparison of value in simple choice , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[3]  D. P. Hanes,et al.  Controlled Movement Processing: Superior Colliculus Activity Associated with Countermanded Saccades , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  D. Vickers Decision processes in visual perception , 1979 .

[5]  Jillian H. Fecteau,et al.  Salience, relevance, and firing: a priority map for target selection , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[6]  Colin Camerer,et al.  A framework for studying the neurobiology of value-based decision making , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[7]  F. Scharnowski,et al.  Long-lasting modulation of feature integration by transcranial magnetic stimulation. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[8]  J. Schall,et al.  Neural Control of Voluntary Movement Initiation , 1996, Science.

[9]  S. Shimojo,et al.  Gaze bias both reflects and influences preference , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[10]  D. Robinson,et al.  Shared neural control of attentional shifts and eye movements , 1996, Nature.

[11]  James L. McClelland,et al.  The time course of perceptual choice: the leaky, competing accumulator model. , 2001, Psychological review.

[12]  Jeremy M. Wolfe,et al.  Guided Search 4.0: Current Progress With a Model of Visual Search , 2007, Integrated Models of Cognitive Systems.

[13]  C. Padoa-Schioppa,et al.  Neurons in the orbitofrontal cortex encode economic value , 2006, Nature.

[14]  Colin Camerer,et al.  The Attentional Drift-Diffusion Model Extends to Simple Purchasing Decisions , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[15]  John K. Tsotsos,et al.  Saliency, attention, and visual search: an information theoretic approach. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[16]  Leslie G. Ungerleider Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .

[17]  To whom correspondence should be addressed; , 2022 .

[18]  C. Koch,et al.  Computational modelling of visual attention , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[19]  C. Koch,et al.  Relative visual saliency differences induce sizable bias in consumer choice , 2012 .

[20]  Jacqueline Gottlieb,et al.  LIP responses to a popout stimulus are reduced if it is overtly ignored , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  Jerome R. Busemeyer,et al.  Computational Models of Decision Making , 2003 .

[22]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Activation of the Human Superior Temporal Gyrus during Observation of Goal Attribution by Intentional Objects , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[23]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Dissociating the Role of the Orbitofrontal Cortex and the Striatum in the Computation of Goal Values and Prediction Errors , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Anatomical constraints on attention: hemifield independence is a signature of multifocal spatial selection. , 2012, Journal of vision.

[25]  C. Bundesen,et al.  A neural theory of visual attention: bridging cognition and neurophysiology. , 2005, Psychological review.

[26]  Bahador Bahrami,et al.  Precision of working memory for visual motion sequences and transparent motion surfaces. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[27]  J. Schall,et al.  Role of frontal eye fields in countermanding saccades: visual, movement, and fixation activity. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Attention, intention, and priority in the parietal lobe. , 2010, Annual review of neuroscience.

[29]  Christof Koch,et al.  A Model of Saliency-Based Visual Attention for Rapid Scene Analysis , 2009 .

[30]  Christof Koch,et al.  The Drift Diffusion Model Can Account for the Accuracy and Reaction Time of Value-Based Choices Under High and Low Time Pressure , 2010, Judgment and Decision Making.

[31]  Liqing Zhang,et al.  Dynamic visual attention: searching for coding length increments , 2008, NIPS.

[32]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  Psychology and neurobiology of simple decisions , 2004, Trends in Neurosciences.

[33]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Orbitofrontal Cortex Encodes Willingness to Pay in Everyday Economic Transactions , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  C. Koch,et al.  Evidence for two distinct mechanisms directing gaze in natural scenes. , 2012, Journal of vision.

[35]  R. Nagel,et al.  Search Dynamics in Consumer Choice under Time Pressure: An Eye-Tracking Study , 2011 .

[36]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  Dual diffusion model for single-cell recording data from the superior colliculus in a brightness-discrimination task. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  Daniel T. Knoepfle,et al.  Value Computations in Ventral Medial Prefrontal Cortex during Charitable Decision Making Incorporate Input from Regions Involved in Social Cognition , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  A. Wald Foundations of a General Theory of Sequential Decision Functions , 1947 .

[39]  R. Andersen,et al.  Cognitive Control Signals for Neural Prosthetics , 2004, Science.

[40]  Nuno Vasconcelos,et al.  The discriminant center-surround hypothesis for bottom-up saliency , 2007, NIPS.

[41]  N R BARTLETT,et al.  Latency and duration of eye movements in the horizontal plane. , 1962, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[42]  Jeffrey D. Schall,et al.  Neural basis of saccade target selection in frontal eye field during visual search , 1993, Nature.

[43]  J. Gold,et al.  The neural basis of decision making. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[44]  Timothy D. Hanks,et al.  Neurobiology of decision making: An intentional framework , 2008 .

[45]  J. Theeuwes Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. , 2010, Acta psychologica.

[46]  M. A. Steinmetz,et al.  Posterior Parietal Cortex Automatically Encodes the Location of Salient Stimuli , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[47]  L. Itti,et al.  Search Goal Tunes Visual Features Optimally , 2007, Neuron.

[48]  R. Jenison,et al.  Value Encoding in Single Neurons in the Human Amygdala during Decision Making , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  P. Glimcher,et al.  The Neurobiology of Decision: Consensus and Controversy , 2009, Neuron.

[50]  A Treisman,et al.  Feature analysis in early vision: evidence from search asymmetries. , 1988, Psychological review.

[51]  J. Townsend,et al.  Decision field theory: a dynamic-cognitive approach to decision making in an uncertain environment. , 1993, Psychological review.

[52]  Ralf van der Lans,et al.  Research Note - Competitive Brand Salience , 2008, Mark. Sci..

[53]  G. Logan,et al.  Inhibitory control in mind and brain: an interactive race model of countermanding saccades. , 2007, Psychological review.

[54]  C. Kennard,et al.  The role of visual salience in directing eye movements in visual object agnosia , 2009, Current Biology.

[55]  M. Goldberg,et al.  The representation of visual salience in monkey parietal cortex , 1998, Nature.

[56]  D. Bouwhuis,et al.  Attention and performance X : control of language processes , 1986 .

[57]  G. Rhodes,et al.  Sex-specific norms code face identity. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[58]  A. Rangel,et al.  Multialternative drift-diffusion model predicts the relationship between visual fixations and choice in value-based decisions , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[59]  Braden A. Purcell,et al.  From Salience to Saccades: Multiple-Alternative Gated Stochastic Accumulator Model of Visual Search , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[60]  Raymond Klein,et al.  Inhibition of return , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[61]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Loss aversion and inhibition in dynamical models of multialternative choice. , 2004, Psychological review.

[62]  M. Posner,et al.  Components of visual orienting , 1984 .

[63]  M. Paré,et al.  Temporal processing of saccade targets in parietal cortex area LIP during visual search. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[64]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  A visual salience map in the primate frontal eye field. , 2005, Progress in brain research.

[65]  Antonio Rangel,et al.  The Decision Value Computations in the vmPFC and Striatum Use a Relative Value Code That is Guided by Visual Attention , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[66]  Christopher Summerfield,et al.  Building Bridges between Perceptual and Economic Decision-Making: Neural and Computational Mechanisms , 2012, Front. Neurosci..

[67]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Effects of similarity and history on neural mechanisms of visual selection , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[68]  Iain D. Gilchrist,et al.  Visual correlates of fixation selection: effects of scale and time , 2005, Vision Research.

[69]  Bijan Pesaran,et al.  Competition for Visual Selection in the Oculomotor System , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[70]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system , 2009, NeuroImage.

[71]  Christof Koch,et al.  Learning a saliency map using fixated locations in natural scenes. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[72]  Christof Koch,et al.  Modeling attention to salient proto-objects , 2006, Neural Networks.

[73]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Activity of superior colliculus in behaving monkey. 3. Cells discharging before eye movements. , 1972, Journal of neurophysiology.

[74]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The physics of optimal decision making: a formal analysis of models of performance in two-alternative forced-choice tasks. , 2006, Psychological review.

[75]  R. Carpenter,et al.  The influence of urgency on decision time , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[76]  Michael L. Platt,et al.  Monkeys Pay Per View: Adaptive Valuation of Social Images by Rhesus Macaques , 2005, Current Biology.

[77]  Erik D. Reichle,et al.  The E-Z Reader model of eye-movement control in reading: Comparisons to other models , 2003, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[78]  P. Montague,et al.  Neural Economics and the Biological Substrates of Valuation , 2002, Neuron.

[79]  Benjamin W Tatler,et al.  The central fixation bias in scene viewing: selecting an optimal viewing position independently of motor biases and image feature distributions. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[80]  Michel Wedel,et al.  Competitive Brand Salience , 2009 .

[81]  Paul W. Glimcher,et al.  Response fields of intraparietal neurons quantified with multiple saccadic targets , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[82]  P. Holmes,et al.  The dynamics of choice among multiple alternatives , 2006 .

[83]  A. Leventhal,et al.  Signal timing across the macaque visual system. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[84]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Estimating parameters of the diffusion model: Approaches to dealing with contaminant reaction times and parameter variability , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[85]  Pietro Perona,et al.  Optimal reward harvesting in complex perceptual environments , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[86]  R H Wurtz,et al.  Organization of monkey superior colliculus: intermediate layer cells discharging before eye movements. , 1976, Journal of neurophysiology.

[87]  W. J. Nowack Methods in Neuronal Modeling , 1991, Neurology.

[88]  Richard P. Heitz,et al.  Neural mechanisms of saccade target selection: gated accumulator model of the visual–motor cascade , 2011, The European journal of neuroscience.

[89]  R. Bogacz Optimal decision-making theories: linking neurobiology with behaviour , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[90]  T. Zandt,et al.  How to fit a response time distribution , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[91]  Idan Segev,et al.  Methods in Neuronal Modeling , 1988 .

[92]  C. Summerfield,et al.  Economic Value Biases Uncertain Perceptual Choices in the Parietal and Prefrontal Cortices , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..