Position encoding of the centres of global structure: Separate form and motion processes

The retinal flow of information during locomotion provides cues to instantaneous heading. Reconciliation of observer trajectory with the internal representation of the environment implies that the positions of the centre of structure of global motion can be localized relative to points in the visual field. Humans are also sensitive to global structure in Glass patterns, which can approximate temporally integrated optic flow. Encoding of the position of centre of structure of Glass patterns could augment the motion information. However, Glass-like pattern structure could also be present in the texture of objects and indicate their centres, raising the question of whether the centres of form and motion patterns are encoded separately. Psychophysical methods are used to examine ability to localize the centres of structure of Glass and global dot motion (GDM) patterns. Radial and concentric Glass patterns are localized more precisely than spiral Glass patterns but performance in localizing the centres of radial, concentric and spiral GDM patterns is equally precise. Also, Glass patterns centres can be localized at signal levels close to their threshold for discrimination from wholly incoherent patterns but GDM pattern centres cannot, suggesting that detectors for looming and rotating stimuli exist that do not rely on the provision of a centre of structure. Collectively, our results provide evidence for independent encoding of the positions of the centres of structure of global motion and form patterns. These positions can be accurately and precisely localized within the visual field. While the centres of structure of a number of form patterns can be simultaneously encoded, allowing their positions to be compared, only a single focus of expansion for optic flow is returned.

[1]  David R. Badcock,et al.  Global motion perception: Interaction of the ON and OFF pathways , 1994, Vision Research.

[2]  Oliver J Braddick,et al.  Psychophysical differences in processing of global motion and form detection and position discrimination. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[3]  Hugh R. Wilson,et al.  A biologically plausible model of human radial frequency perception , 2006, Vision Research.

[4]  F. Bremmer,et al.  Perception of self-motion from visual flow , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  David R Badcock,et al.  Asymmetries in the Sensitivity to Motion in Depth: A Centripetal Bias , 1993, Perception.

[6]  Laurence R. Harris,et al.  Seeing spatial form , 2005 .

[7]  Julie M. Harris,et al.  Guidance of locomotion on foot uses perceived target location rather than optic flow , 1998, Current Biology.

[8]  H. Wilson,et al.  Detection of global structure in Glass patterns: implications for form vision , 1998, Vision Research.

[9]  D. Burr,et al.  Two stages of visual processing for radial and circular motion , 1995, Nature.

[10]  D. Regan,et al.  Visual processing of four kinds of relative motion , 1986, Vision Research.

[11]  W H Warren,et al.  Perceiving Heading in the Presence of Moving Objects , 1995, Perception.

[12]  Takeo Watanabe,et al.  High-Level Motion Processing , 1998 .

[13]  K. Rayner Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. , 1998, Psychological bulletin.

[14]  K Prazdny,et al.  On the Perception of Glass Patterns , 1984, Perception.

[15]  H. Barlow,et al.  Convergent evidence for the visual analysis of optic flow through anisotropic attenuation of high spatial frequencies. , 2004, Journal of vision.

[16]  Jitendra Malik,et al.  Contour and Texture Analysis for Image Segmentation , 2001, International Journal of Computer Vision.

[17]  D Regan,et al.  Visually guided locomotion: psychophysical evidence for a neural mechanism sensitive to flow patterns. , 1979, Science.

[18]  Mark Mon-Williams,et al.  The role of serotonin in visuomotor activity , 2004 .

[19]  S. Dakin,et al.  Local and global visual grouping: tuning for spatial frequency and contrast. , 2001, Journal of vision.

[20]  David R. Badcock,et al.  Selectivity for coherence in polar orientation in human form vision , 2007, Vision Research.

[21]  J. Koenderink,et al.  Differential spatial displacement discrimination thresholds for Gabor patches , 1988, Vision Research.

[22]  D. Burr,et al.  Large receptive fields for optic flow detection in humans , 1998, Vision Research.

[23]  Marie-Juliette F Mandelli,et al.  The local and global processing of chromatic Glass patterns. , 2005, Journal of vision.

[24]  D Regan,et al.  How do we avoid confounding the direction we are looking and the direction we are moving? , 1982, Science.

[25]  David R. Badcock,et al.  Interactions between luminance and contrast signals in global form detection , 2005, Vision Research.

[26]  L. Glass,et al.  Pattern Recognition in Humans: Correlations Which Cannot be Perceived , 1976, Perception.

[27]  David C. Burr,et al.  Cardinal axes for radial and circular motion, revealed by summation and by masking , 2001, Vision Research.

[28]  Vision Research , 1961, Nature.

[29]  Lindsay T Sharpe,et al.  The dependence of luminous efficiency on chromatic adaptation. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[30]  R. Pérez,et al.  Perception of Random Dot Interference Patterns , 1973, Nature.

[31]  S. Watamaniuk High-Level Motion Processing: Computational, Neurobiological, and Psychophysical Perspectives , 1999 .

[32]  David Whitaker,et al.  Neither here nor there: localizing conflicting visual attributes. , 2003, Journal of vision.

[33]  Vincent P Ferrera,et al.  Detection thresholds for spiral Glass patterns , 2001, Vision Research.

[34]  Colin W. G. Clifford,et al.  The Inputs to Global Form Detection , 2006 .

[35]  H. Wilson,et al.  Concentric orientation summation in human form vision , 1997, Vision Research.

[36]  Wilson S. Geisler,et al.  Motion streaks provide a spatial code for motion direction , 1999, Nature.

[37]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Sensitivity of MST neurons to optic flow stimuli. I. A continuum of response selectivity to large-field stimuli. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  William H. Warren,et al.  Optic flow is used to control human walking , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[39]  Frank Bremmer,et al.  Neural correlates of implied motion , 2003, Nature.

[40]  R. Hetherington The Perception of the Visual World , 1952 .

[41]  Robert Sekuler,et al.  Coherent global motion percepts from stochastic local motions , 1984, Vision Research.

[42]  L. Glass Moiré Effect from Random Dots , 1969, Nature.

[43]  C. Baker,et al.  Residual motion perception in a "motion-blind" patient, assessed with limited-lifetime random dot stimuli , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[44]  David R. Badcock,et al.  Coherent global motion in the absence of coherent velocity signals , 2000, Current Biology.

[45]  W. H. Warren The state of flow , 1998 .

[46]  Neil W Roach,et al.  Masking exposes multiple global form mechanisms. , 2008, Journal of vision.