Global shape versus local feature: An angle illusion

We have shown previously that the precision of angle judgments depends strongly on the global stimulus configuration: discrimination thresholds for angles that form part of isosceles triangles are up to 3 times lower than for those that form part of scalene triangles [Kennedy, G. J., Orbach, H. S., & Loffler, G. (2006). Effects of global shape on angle discrimination. VisionResearch, 46(8-9), 1530-1539]. Here, we investigated whether or not the perceived size of an angle (accuracy) is also affected by the overall shape of which it forms a part. Observers compared the relative sizes of angles contained in isosceles triangles with those of angles in scalene triangles and points of subjective equality were determined. For a reference angle of 60 degrees , angles embedded in isosceles triangles were judged to be on average 14 degrees larger than angles embedded in scalene triangles. This result is largely independent of the reference angle, triangle orientation and triangle size. Moreover, the effect is present whether or not triangles of different shapes enclose the same area, whether or not the side of the triangle opposite the angle is present and whether the triangle is outlined or defined by dots at its vertexes. In sum, our results provide evidence for a novel illusion where an angle embedded in an isosceles triangle is judged substantially larger than the same angle embedded in a scalene triangle. This finding demonstrates that mechanisms for computing angles are sensitive to the context within which angles are presented.

[1]  N. Graham,et al.  Detection of grating patterns containing two spatial frequencies: a comparison of single-channel and multiple-channels models. , 1971, Vision research.

[2]  H. Bouma,et al.  Induced changes in the perceived orientation of line segments. , 1970, Vision research.

[3]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex , 1962, The Journal of physiology.

[4]  J J Koenderink,et al.  Visual Size Invariance Does Not Apply to Geometric Angle and Speed of Rotation , 1993, Perception.

[5]  J. M. Zanker,et al.  How To Tell Circles from Ellipses: Perceiving the Regularity of Simple Shapes , 1999, Naturwissenschaften.

[6]  D. W. Heeley,et al.  Mechanisms Specialized for the Perception of Image Geometry , 1996, Vision Research.

[7]  D. Perrett,et al.  Visual neurones responsive to faces in the monkey temporal cortex , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  D Purves,et al.  Why are angles misperceived? , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[9]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[10]  R. Gregory,et al.  Distortion of Visual Space as Inappropriate Constancy Scaling , 1963, Nature.

[11]  Minami Ito,et al.  Representation of Angles Embedded within Contour Stimuli in Area V2 of Macaque Monkeys , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  D. Regan,et al.  Postadaptation orientation discrimination. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[13]  D. Regan,et al.  Shape discrimination and the judgement of perfect symmetry: Dissociation of shape from size , 1992, Vision Research.

[14]  C. Connor,et al.  Responses to contour features in macaque area V4. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  R. Desimone,et al.  Stimulus-selective properties of inferior temporal neurons in the macaque , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[16]  J. O. Robinson The Psychology of Visual Illusion , 1972 .

[17]  J. Jastrow Studies from the University of Wisconsin: On the Judgment of Angles and Positions of Lines , 1892 .

[18]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Neuronal selectivities to complex object features in the ventral visual pathway of the macaque cerebral cortex. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  Gunter Loffler,et al.  Effects of global shape on angle discrimination , 2006, Vision Research.

[20]  Johan Wagemans,et al.  Characteristics and models of human symmetry detection , 1997, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[21]  P. O. Bishop,et al.  Spatial vision. , 1971, Annual review of psychology.

[22]  M J Morgan,et al.  The visual computation of 2-D area by human observers , 2005, Vision Research.

[23]  Leslie G. Ungerleider Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .

[24]  J. Robson,et al.  Application of fourier analysis to the visibility of gratings , 1968, The Journal of physiology.

[25]  J. Hegdé,et al.  Selectivity for Complex Shapes in Primate Visual Area V2 , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  D. C. Essen,et al.  Neural responses to polar, hyperbolic, and Cartesian gratings in area V4 of the macaque monkey. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  D. Regan Visual information channeling in normal and disordered vision. , 1982, Psychological review.

[28]  Dennis M. Levi,et al.  Angle judgment: Is the whole the sum of its parts? , 1996, Vision Research.

[29]  F. Attneave Some informational aspects of visual perception. , 1954, Psychological review.

[30]  J J Koenderink,et al.  Discrimination of geometric angle in the fronto-parallel plane. , 1994, Spatial vision.

[31]  R. Weale Analysis of Visual Behaviour , 1983 .

[32]  P Wenderoth,et al.  The effects of angle-arm length on judgments of angle magnitude and orientation contrast , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.

[33]  D. Regan,et al.  Evidence for a neural mechanism that encodes angles , 1996, Vision Research.

[34]  E. Poulton Models for biases in judging sensory magnitude. , 1979, Psychological bulletin.

[35]  I. Biederman Recognition-by-components: a theory of human image understanding. , 1987, Psychological review.

[36]  C. Blakemore,et al.  Lateral Inhibition between Orientation Detectors in the Human Visual System , 1970, Nature.

[37]  M. Hayhoe,et al.  Integration of Form across Saccadic Eye Movements , 1991, Perception.

[38]  D. V. Essen,et al.  Neural mechanisms of form and motion processing in the primate visual system , 1994, Neuron.

[39]  Vision Research , 1961, Nature.

[40]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex , 1968, The Journal of physiology.

[41]  E. Greene,et al.  Angular Induction as a Function of the Length and Position of Segments and Gaps , 1994, Perception.

[42]  Quick Rf A vector-magnitude model of contrast detection. , 1974 .

[43]  H. Barlow,et al.  The versatility and absolute efficiency of detecting mirror symmetry in random dot displays , 1979, Vision Research.

[44]  W. Richards,et al.  Mechanisms of contour curvature discrimination. , 1989, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[45]  M. Goodale,et al.  Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand , 1995, Current Biology.