A functional angle on some after-effects in cortical vision

The question of how our brains and those of other animals code sensory information is of fundamental importance to neuroscience research. Visual illusions offer valuable insight into the mechanisms of perceptual coding. One such illusion, the tilt after–effect (TAE), has been studied extensively since the 1930s, yet a full explanation of the effect has remained elusive. Here, we put forward an explanation of the TAE in terms of a functional role for adaptation in the visual cortex. The proposed model accounts not only for the phenomenology of the TAE, but also for spatial interactions in perceived tilt and the effects of adaptation on the perception of direction of motion and colour. We discuss the implications of the model for understanding the effects of adaptation and surround stimulation on the response properties of cortical neurons.

[1]  J. Gibson,et al.  Adaptation, after-effect and contrast in the perception of tilted lines. I. Quantitative studies , 1937 .

[2]  H. Wallach,et al.  Figural aftereffects; an investigation of visual processes. , 1944 .

[3]  F. Attneave Some informational aspects of visual perception. , 1954, Psychological review.

[4]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex , 1962, The Journal of physiology.

[5]  D. P. Andrews Error-correcting Perceptual Mechanisms , 1964 .

[6]  J. R. Harris,et al.  TWO DIFFERENT AFTER-EFFECTS OF EXPOSURE TO VISUAL TILTS. , 1965, The American journal of psychology.

[7]  M. Georgeson,et al.  Spatial Frequency Selectivity of a Visual Tilt Illusion , 1973, Nature.

[8]  D. Mitchell,et al.  The spatial selectivity of the tilt aftereffect. , 1974, Vision research.

[9]  D. Mitchell,et al.  Does the tilt after-effect occur in the oblique meridian? , 1976, Vision Research.

[10]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Adaptation alters perceived direction of motion , 1976, Vision Research.

[11]  Peter Wenderoth,et al.  The tilt illusion: Repulsion and attraction effects in the oblique meridian , 1977, Vision Research.

[12]  R. M. Boynton,et al.  Chromaticity diagram showing cone excitation by stimuli of equal luminance. , 1979, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[13]  G. Mather The Movement Aftereffect and a Distribution-Shift Model for Coding the Direction of Visual Movement , 1980, Perception.

[14]  D. W. Heeley,et al.  Cardinal directions of color space , 1982, Vision Research.

[15]  S. Laughlin,et al.  Predictive coding: a fresh view of inhibition in the retina , 1982, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[16]  S. Zeki Colour coding in the cerebral cortex: The reaction of cells in monkey visual cortex to wavelengths and colours , 1983, Neuroscience.

[17]  S. Zeki Colour coding in the cerebral cortex: The responses of wavelength-selective and colour-coded cells in monkey visual cortex to changes in wavelength composition , 1983, Neuroscience.

[18]  R. Desimone,et al.  Columnar organization of directionally selective cells in visual area MT of the macaque. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  P. Lennie,et al.  Chromatic mechanisms in lateral geniculate nucleus of macaque. , 1984, The Journal of physiology.

[20]  J Allman,et al.  Direction- and Velocity-Specific Responses from beyond the Classical Receptive Field in the Middle Temporal Visual Area (MT) , 1985, Perception.

[21]  P Wenderoth,et al.  Possible Neural Substrates for Orientation Analysis and Perception , 1987, Perception.

[22]  P. Wenderoth,et al.  The different mechanisms of the direct and indirect tilt illusions , 1988, Vision Research.

[23]  Simon B. Laughlin,et al.  Coding Efficiency and Design in Visual Processing , 1989 .

[24]  Peter Földiák,et al.  Adaptation and decorrelation in the cortex , 1989 .

[25]  T. Wiesel,et al.  The influence of contextual stimuli on the orientation selectivity of cells in primary visual cortex of the cat , 1990, Vision Research.

[26]  Gerald Westhhmer,et al.  Simultaneous orientation contrast for lines in the human fovea , 1990, Vision Research.

[27]  G Westheimer,et al.  Simultaneous orientation contrast for lines in the human fovea. , 1990, Vision research.

[28]  E. Adelson,et al.  The Plenoptic Function and the Elements of Early Vision , 1991 .

[29]  M. Webster,et al.  Changes in colour appearance following post-receptoral adaptation , 1991, Nature.

[30]  Zhaoping Li,et al.  What does post-adaptation color appearance reveal about cortical color representation? , 1993, Vision Research.

[31]  M. Webster,et al.  The influence of contrast adaptation on color appearance , 1994, Vision Research.

[32]  M. Carandini,et al.  Summation and division by neurons in primate visual cortex. , 1994, Science.

[33]  S. Nelson,et al.  An emergent model of orientation selectivity in cat visual cortical simple cells , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[34]  Robert Patterson,et al.  Direction-selective adaptation and simultaneous contrast induced by stereoscopic (cyclopean) motion , 1996, Vision Research.

[35]  V. Mountcastle The columnar organization of the neocortex. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[36]  Peter E. Latham,et al.  Statistically Efficient Estimation Using Population Coding , 1998, Neural Computation.

[37]  N. Draper,et al.  Applied Regression Analysis: Draper/Applied Regression Analysis , 1998 .

[38]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  Local velocity representation: evidence from motion adaptation , 1998, Vision Research.

[39]  Martin J. Wainwright,et al.  Visual adaptation as optimal information transmission , 1999, Vision Research.

[40]  Richard F. Gunst,et al.  Applied Regression Analysis , 1999, Technometrics.

[41]  P. Lennie,et al.  Rapid adaptation in visual cortex to the structure of images. , 1999, Science.