Childhood as a solution to explore–exploit tensions

I argue that the evolution of our life history, with its distinctively long, protected human childhood, allows an early period of broad hypothesis search and exploration, before the demands of goal-directed exploitation set in. This cognitive profile is also found in other animals and is associated with early behaviours such as neophilia and play. I relate this developmental pattern to computational ideas about explore–exploit trade-offs, search and sampling, and to neuroscience findings. I also present several lines of empirical evidence suggesting that young human learners are highly exploratory, both in terms of their search for external information and their search through hypothesis spaces. In fact, they are sometimes more exploratory than older learners and adults. This article is part of the theme issue ‘Life history and learning: how childhood, caregiving and old age shape cognition and culture in humans and other animals’.

[1]  M. Tomasello The adaptive origins of uniquely human sociality , 2020, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B.

[2]  K. Hawkes Cognitive consequences of our grandmothering life history: cultural learning begins in infancy , 2020, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B.

[3]  Judith M Burkart,et al.  The emergence of emotionally modern humans: implications for language and learning , 2020, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B.

[4]  Alison Gopnik,et al.  Shake it baby, but only when needed: Preschoolers adapt their exploratory strategies to the information structure of the task , 2019, Cognition.

[5]  Catherine A. Hartley,et al.  Reinforcement learning across development: What insights can we draw from a decade of research? , 2019, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[6]  N. Tottenham,et al.  Parental presence switches avoidance to attraction learning in children , 2019, Nature Human Behaviour.

[7]  Caren M. Walker,et al.  Causal Learning Across Culture and Socioeconomic Status. , 2019, Child development.

[8]  Samuel J. Gershman,et al.  The algorithmic architecture of exploration in the human brain , 2019, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[9]  Amy Perfors,et al.  The Exploration Advantage: Children’s instinct to explore allows them to find information that adults miss. , 2019 .

[10]  L. Aplin Culture and cultural evolution in birds: a review of the evidence , 2019, Animal Behaviour.

[11]  Sean C. Murphy,et al.  Creation Across Culture: Children’s Tool Innovation Is Influenced by Cultural and Developmental Factors , 2019, Developmental psychology.

[12]  Alexander S. Rich,et al.  The Limits of Learning: Exploration, Generalization, and the Development of Learning Traps , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[13]  Charley M. Wu,et al.  Searching for Rewards Like a Child Means Less Generalization and More Directed Exploration , 2018, bioRxiv.

[14]  S. Aitken The Gardener and the Carpenter: What the New Science of Child Development Tells Us About the Relationship Between Parents and Children , 2018 .

[15]  Thomas N. Sherratt,et al.  The adaptive significance of age-dependent changes in the tendency of individuals to explore , 2018, Animal Behaviour.

[16]  A. Rosati Foraging Cognition: Reviving the Ecological Intelligence Hypothesis , 2017, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[17]  Mark O’Hara,et al.  Object play in parrots and corvids , 2017, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

[18]  Brendan J. Barrett,et al.  Older, sociable capuchins (Cebus capucinus) invent more social behaviors, but younger monkeys innovate more in other contexts , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[19]  Christopher G. Lucas,et al.  Changes in cognitive flexibility and hypothesis search across human life history from childhood to adolescence to adulthood , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[20]  Daniel J. Plebanek,et al.  Corrigendum: Costs of Selective Attention: When Children Notice What Adults Miss , 2017, Psychological science.

[21]  Daniel J. Plebanek,et al.  Costs of Selective Attention: When Children Notice What Adults Miss , 2017, Psychological science.

[22]  Fei Xu,et al.  Learning Higher-Order Generalizations Through Free Play: Evidence From 2- and 3-Year-Old Children , 2017, Developmental psychology.

[23]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Great apes anticipate that other individuals will act according to false beliefs , 2016, Science.

[24]  Jason G. Goldman,et al.  Enhanced learning of natural visual sequences in newborn chicks , 2016, Animal Cognition.

[25]  Harvey Whitehouse,et al.  Imitative flexibility and the development of cultural learning , 2015, Cognition.

[26]  W. Frankenhuis,et al.  Adaptive explanations for sensitive windows in development , 2015, Frontiers in Zoology.

[27]  Steven Hamblin,et al.  Taking the Operant Paradigm into the Field: Associative Learning in Wild Great Tits , 2015, PloS one.

[28]  Aimee E. Stahl,et al.  Observing the unexpected enhances infants’ learning and exploration , 2015, Science.

[29]  Christopher G. Lucas,et al.  When Younger Learners Can Be Better (or at Least More Open-Minded) Than Older Ones , 2015 .

[30]  P. Gunz,et al.  Brain ontogeny and life history in Pleistocene hominins , 2015, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[31]  Barbara L. Finlay,et al.  Developmental mechanisms channeling cortical evolution , 2015, Trends in Neurosciences.

[32]  Takao K Hensch,et al.  Critical periods in speech perception: new directions. , 2015, Annual review of psychology.

[33]  A. Thornton,et al.  Experimentally induced innovations lead to persistent culture via conformity in wild birds , 2014, Nature.

[34]  Thomas L. Griffiths,et al.  Win-Stay, Lose-Sample: A simple sequential algorithm for approximating Bayesian inference , 2014, Cognitive Psychology.

[35]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Humans use directed and random exploration to solve the explore-exploit dilemma. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[36]  S. Denison,et al.  Probabilistic models, learning algorithms, and response variability: sampling in cognitive development , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[37]  P. Hof,et al.  Metabolic costs and evolutionary implications of human brain development , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[38]  Christopher G. Lucas,et al.  When children are better (or at least more open-minded) learners than adults: Developmental differences in learning the forms of causal relationships , 2014, Cognition.

[39]  M. Bikson,et al.  Noninvasive transcranial direct current stimulation over the left prefrontal cortex facilitates cognitive flexibility in tool use , 2013, Cognitive neuroscience.

[40]  E. Snell-Rood,et al.  An overview of the evolutionary causes and consequences of behavioural plasticity , 2013, Animal Behaviour.

[41]  Jorgelina María Guido,et al.  The Effect of Age and Sex on Object Exploration and Manipulative Behavior in a Neotropical Raptor, the Chimango Caracara, Milvago chimango , 2013 .

[42]  Noah D. Goodman,et al.  Did she jump because she was the big sister or because the trampoline was safe? Causal inference and the development of social attribution. , 2013, Child development.

[43]  S. Denison,et al.  Rational variability in children’s causal inferences: The Sampling Hypothesis , 2013, Cognition.

[44]  Sarah Benson-Amram,et al.  Innovative problem solving by wild spotted hyenas , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[45]  E. Crone,et al.  Understanding adolescence as a period of social–affective engagement and goal flexibility , 2012, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[46]  A. Gopnik,et al.  The power of possibility: causal learning, counterfactual reasoning, and pretend play , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[47]  L. Schulz The origins of inquiry: inductive inference and exploration in early childhood , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[48]  S. Herculano‐Houzel The remarkable, yet not extraordinary, human brain as a scaled-up primate brain and its associated cost , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[49]  L. Schulz,et al.  Children balance theories and evidence in exploration, explanation, and learning , 2012, Cognitive Psychology.

[50]  P. L. Harris Trusting What You’re Told , 2012 .

[51]  A. Gopnik,et al.  Reconstructing constructivism: causal models, Bayesian learning mechanisms, and the theory theory. , 2012, Psychological bulletin.

[52]  W. Frankenhuis,et al.  Balancing sampling and specialization: an adaptationist model of incremental development , 2011, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[53]  John L. Quinn,et al.  Who are the innovators? A field experiment with 2 passerine species , 2011 .

[54]  Linda Wilbrecht,et al.  Juvenile mice show greater flexibility in multiple choice reversal learning than adults , 2011, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[55]  A. Gopnik,et al.  Children’s imitation of causal action sequences is influenced by statistical and pedagogical evidence , 2011, Cognition.

[56]  Noah D. Goodman,et al.  Where science starts: Spontaneous experiments in preschoolers’ exploratory play , 2011, Cognition.

[57]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Own and Others' Prior Experiences Influence Children's Imitation of Causal Acts. , 2011, Cognitive development.

[58]  Mark Nielsen,et al.  Imitation in young children: when who gets copied is more important than what gets copied. , 2011, Developmental psychology.

[59]  S. Hrdy,et al.  Mothers and Others , 2009 .

[60]  D. Papaj,et al.  Reproductive tradeoffs of learning in a butterfly , 2011 .

[61]  Tanya M. Smith,et al.  Dental evidence for ontogenetic differences between modern humans and Neanderthals , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[62]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Dynamic reconfiguration of human brain networks during learning , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[63]  V. Weisbecker,et al.  Brain size, life history, and metabolism at the marsupial/placental dichotomy , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[64]  Russell D Gray,et al.  Social learning in new Caledonian crows , 2010, Learning & behavior.

[65]  Laura Marina Biondi,et al.  Inter-individual and age differences in exploration, neophobia and problem-solving ability in a Neotropical raptor (Milvago chimango) , 2010, Animal Cognition.

[66]  J. Tenenbaum,et al.  A probabilistic model of theory formation , 2010, Cognition.

[67]  Michael Ramscar,et al.  Cognition Without Control When a Little Frontal Lobe Goes a Long Way , 2009 .

[68]  P. Harvey,et al.  Brain size, development and metabolism in birds and mammals , 2009 .

[69]  Alex Kacelnik,et al.  Tools for thought or thoughts for tools? , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[70]  Gordon M. Burghardt,et al.  The Genesis of Animal Play , 2009 .

[71]  M. Rowland The Secret of Our Success. , 2008 .

[72]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[73]  L. Schulz,et al.  Serious fun: preschoolers engage in more exploratory play when evidence is confounded. , 2007, Developmental psychology.

[74]  C. Glymour,et al.  Preschool children learn about causal structure from conditional interventions. , 2007, Developmental science.

[75]  Brian Hare,et al.  From Nonhuman to Human Mind , 2007 .

[76]  S. Moriceau,et al.  Maternal presence serves as a switch between learning fear and attraction in infancy , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[77]  M. Gurven,et al.  Energetic demand of multiple dependents and the evolution of slow human growth , 2006, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[78]  Julia E. Meyers-Manor The genesis of animal play: Testing the limits , 2005 .

[79]  S. M. Carlson Developmentally Sensitive Measures of Executive Function in Preschool Children , 2005, Developmental neuropsychology.

[80]  A. Whiten,et al.  Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens) , 2005, Animal Cognition.

[81]  K. Laland,et al.  Age differences in neophilia, exploration, and innovation in family groups of callitrichid monkeys , 2005, American journal of primatology.

[82]  P. Kuhl Early language acquisition: cracking the speech code , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[83]  Y. Munakata,et al.  Developmental cognitive neuroscience: progress and potential , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[84]  G. Hunt,et al.  Diversification and cumulative evolution in New Caledonian crow tool manufacture , 2003, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[85]  J. Stevenson The cultural origins of human cognition , 2001 .

[86]  H. Whitehead,et al.  Culture in whales and dolphins , 2001, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[87]  T. German,et al.  Immunity to functional fixedness in young children , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[88]  Hoon Kim,et al.  Monte Carlo Statistical Methods , 2000, Technometrics.

[89]  S. Carey The Origin of Concepts , 2000 .

[90]  E. Charnov,et al.  Grandmothering, menopause, and the evolution of human life histories. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[91]  Richard Potts,et al.  Evolution and Climate Variability , 1996, Science.

[92]  S. Gelman,et al.  Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity In Cognition And Culture , 1994 .

[93]  A. Gopnik,et al.  The theory theory. , 1994 .

[94]  H. Wellman,et al.  Insides and essences: Early understandings of the non-obvious , 1991, Cognition.

[95]  P. Huttenlocher Morphometric study of human cerebral cortex development , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[96]  R. Byrne,et al.  Machiavellian intelligence : social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans , 1990 .

[97]  C. D. Gelatt,et al.  Optimization by Simulated Annealing , 1983, Science.

[98]  L. Klotz New Directions , 1979, The Canadian journal of urology.

[99]  J. Bruner Nature and uses of immaturity. , 1972 .

[100]  Supplemental Material for Creation Across Culture: Children’s Tool Innovation Is Influenced by Cultural and Developmental Factors , 2019, Developmental Psychology.

[101]  Rommy von Bernhardi,et al.  What Is Neural Plasticity? , 2017, Advances in experimental medicine and biology.

[102]  Mario Baum,et al.  Culture And The Evolutionary Process , 2016 .

[103]  A. Vassallo,et al.  The role of stimulus complexity, age and experience in the expression of exploratory behaviour in the Chimango Caracara, Milvago chimango , 2014, Animal Cognition.

[104]  C. Legare Exploring explanation: explaining inconsistent evidence informs exploratory, hypothesis-testing behavior in young children. , 2012, Child development.

[105]  Mirko Farina The Evolved Apprentice , 2012 .

[106]  Eduardo F. Morales,et al.  An Introduction to Reinforcement Learning , 2011 .

[107]  T. Bergman,et al.  Comparing responses to novel objects in wild baboons (Papio ursinus) and geladas (Theropithecus gelada) , 2008, Animal Cognition.

[108]  B. Chapais Primeval Kinship: How Pair-Bonding Gave Birth to Human Society , 2008 .

[109]  Katherine D. Kinzler,et al.  Core knowledge. , 2007, Developmental science.

[110]  David M. Sobel,et al.  A theory of causal learning in children: causal maps and Bayes nets. , 2004, Psychological review.

[111]  Kim Hill,et al.  A theory of human life history evolution: Diet, intelligence, and longevity , 2000 .

[112]  D. Buonomano,et al.  Cortical plasticity: from synapses to maps. , 1998, Annual review of neuroscience.

[113]  B. Bogin,et al.  Evolutionary hypotheses for human childhood , 1997 .

[114]  B. L. Green,et al.  The Adaptive Nature of Cognitive Immaturity. , 1992 .

[115]  Giacomo Mauro DAriano Advances in child development and behavior. , 1991, Advances in child development and behavior.

[116]  Lawrence. Davis,et al.  Handbook Of Genetic Algorithms , 1990 .

[117]  H. Kelley The processes of causal attribution. , 1973 .