Brains striving for coherence: Long-term cumulative plot formation in the default mode network

Many everyday activities, such as engaging in conversation or listening to a story, require us to sustain attention over a prolonged period of time while integrating and synthesizing complex episodic content into a coherent mental model. Humans are remarkably capable of navigating and keeping track of all the parallel social activities of everyday life even when confronted with interruptions or changes in the environment. However, the underlying cognitive and neurocognitive mechanisms of such long-term integration and profiling of information remain a challenge to neuroscience. While brain activity is generally traceable within the short time frame of working memory (milliseconds to seconds), these integrative processes last for minutes, hours or even days. Here we report two experiments on story comprehension. Experiment I establishes a cognitive dissociation between our comprehension of plot and incidental facts in narratives: when episodic material allows for long-term integration in a coherent plot, we recall fewer factual details. However, when plot formation is challenged, we pay more attention to incidental facts. Experiment II investigates the neural underpinnings of plot formation. Results suggest a central role for the brain's default mode network related to comprehension of coherent narratives while incoherent episodes rather activate the frontoparietal control network. Moreover, an analysis of cortical activity as a function of the cumulative integration of narrative material into a coherent story reveals to linear modulations of right hemisphere posterior temporal and parietal regions. Together these findings point to key neural mechanisms involved in the fundamental human capacity for cumulative plot formation.

[1]  R. Buckner,et al.  Evidence for the Default Network's Role in Spontaneous Cognition , 2010 .

[2]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Wandering Minds: The Default Network and Stimulus-Independent Thought , 2007, Science.

[3]  D. Heeger,et al.  A Hierarchy of Temporal Receptive Windows in Human Cortex , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  Maurizio Corbetta,et al.  The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[5]  P. Dayan,et al.  Cortical substrates for exploratory decisions in humans , 2006, Nature.

[6]  R. N. Spreng,et al.  The Future of Memory: Remembering, Imagining, and the Brain , 2012, Neuron.

[7]  Jiang Xu,et al.  Language in context: emergent features of word, sentence, and narrative comprehension , 2005, NeuroImage.

[8]  James K. Kroger,et al.  Recruitment of anterior dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in human reasoning: a parametric study of relational complexity. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[9]  G H Glover,et al.  Image‐based method for retrospective correction of physiological motion effects in fMRI: RETROICOR , 2000, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[10]  Kevin S. Brown,et al.  Cooperation between the default mode network and the frontal–parietal network in the production of an internal train of thought , 2012, Brain Research.

[11]  Demis Hassabis,et al.  The construction system of the brain , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[12]  J. Smallwood,et al.  When attention matters: The curious incident of the wandering mind , 2008, Memory & cognition.

[13]  Demis Hassabis,et al.  Differential engagement of brain regions within a ‘core’ network during scene construction , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  L. Talmy Toward a Cognitive Semantics , 2003 .

[15]  Kristina M. Visscher,et al.  A Core System for the Implementation of Task Sets , 2006, Neuron.

[16]  Morton Ann Gernsbacher,et al.  Managing Mental Representations During Narrative Comprehension , 2004, Discourse processes.

[17]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  Independent component model of the default-mode brain function: Assessing the impact of active thinking , 2006, Brain Research Bulletin.

[18]  D. Schacter,et al.  The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: remembering the past and imagining the future , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[19]  A. Goldman To Appear in: , 2008 .

[20]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Movement‐Related effects in fMRI time‐series , 1996, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[21]  R. Buckner,et al.  Functional-Anatomic Fractionation of the Brain's Default Network , 2010, Neuron.

[22]  David Poeppel,et al.  The analysis of speech in different temporal integration windows: cerebral lateralization as 'asymmetric sampling in time' , 2003, Speech Commun..

[23]  D. Hassabis,et al.  Deconstructing episodic memory with construction , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[24]  W. Penny,et al.  Random-Effects Analysis , 2002 .

[25]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[26]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Solving future problems: Default network and executive activity associated with goal-directed mental simulations , 2011, NeuroImage.

[27]  Elizabeth R. Chrastil,et al.  Sustained neural activity associated with cognitive control during temporally extended decision making. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[28]  Randy L. Buckner,et al.  2 Evidence for the Default Network's Role in Spontaneous Cognition 3 4 5 , 2010 .

[29]  Andreas Roepstorff,et al.  Theory of Mind in adults with right hemisphere damage: What’s the story? , 2010, Brain and Language.

[30]  S. Young,et al.  Medial and Lateral of the , 2014 .

[31]  Krzysztof J. Gorgolewski,et al.  Medial and Lateral Networks in Anterior Prefrontal Cortex Support Metacognitive Ability for Memory and Perception , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  Haakon G. Engen,et al.  Escaping the here and now: Evidence for a role of the default mode network in perceptually decoupled thought , 2013, NeuroImage.

[33]  S. Ishii,et al.  Resolution of Uncertainty in Prefrontal Cortex , 2006, Neuron.

[34]  Andrea Bergmann,et al.  Statistical Parametric Mapping The Analysis Of Functional Brain Images , 2016 .

[35]  M. Donald,et al.  The slow process: A hypothetical cognitive adaptation for distributed cognitive networks , 2007, Journal of Physiology-Paris.

[36]  R. Nathan Spreng,et al.  The Common Neural Basis of Autobiographical Memory, Prospection, Navigation, Theory of Mind, and the Default Mode: A Quantitative Meta-analysis , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[37]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Non-white noise in fMRI: Does modelling have an impact? , 2006, NeuroImage.

[38]  M. Kutas,et al.  Semantic integration in reading: engagement of the right hemisphere during discourse processing. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[39]  D. Schacter,et al.  The Brain's Default Network , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[40]  E. Crone,et al.  Neural evidence for dissociable components of task-switching. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[41]  B. Harrison,et al.  Consistency and functional specialization in the default mode brain network , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[42]  D. Rubin,et al.  The spatiotemporal dynamics of autobiographical memory: neural correlates of recall, emotional intensity, and reliving. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[43]  Charan Ranganath,et al.  Prefrontal Cortex and Long-Term Memory Encoding: An Integrative Review of Findings from Neuropsychology and Neuroimaging , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[44]  E. Koechlin,et al.  Anterior Prefrontal Function and the Limits of Human Decision-Making , 2007, Science.

[45]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  The Construction of Situation Models in Narrative Comprehension: An Event-Indexing Model , 1995 .

[46]  Randy L. Buckner,et al.  Functional–Anatomic Correlates of Sustained and Transient Processing Components Engaged during Controlled Retrieval , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[47]  W. D. Penny,et al.  Random-Effects Analysis , 2002 .

[48]  E. Koechlin,et al.  The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in human cognition , 1999, Nature.

[49]  Elliot T. Berkman,et al.  Evidence for social working memory from a parametric functional MRI study , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[50]  C. Honey,et al.  Topographic Mapping of a Hierarchy of Temporal Receptive Windows Using a Narrated Story , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  J. Smallwood Distinguishing how from why the mind wanders: a process-occurrence framework for self-generated mental activity. , 2013, Psychological bulletin.

[52]  J. Andrews-Hanna,et al.  The brain's default network: Anatomy, function, and consequence of disruption , 2009 .

[53]  A. Baddeley Working memory: looking back and looking forward , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[54]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Evidence for a frontoparietal control system revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[55]  Uta Frith,et al.  Theory of mind , 2001, Current Biology.

[56]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .