Category coherence and category-based property induction

One important property of human object categories is that they define the sets of exemplars to which newly observed properties are generalized. We manipulated the causal knowledge associated with novel categories and assessed the resulting strength of property inductions. We found that the theoretical coherence afforded to a category by inter-feature causal relationships strengthened inductive projections. However, this effect depended on the degree to which the exemplar with the to-be-projected predicate manifested or satisfied its category's causal laws. That is, the coherence that supports inductive generalizations is a property of individual category members rather than categories. Moreover, we found that an exemplar's coherence was mediated by its degree of category membership. These results were obtained across a variety of causal network topologies and kinds of categories, including biological kinds, non-living natural kinds, and artifacts.

[1]  Variability and Confirmation , 1993 .

[2]  L. Brooks,et al.  Category organization in free classification: The organizing effect of an array of stimuli. , 1995 .

[3]  W. Ahn,et al.  Causal Status as a Determinant of Feature Centrality , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[4]  Thomas L. Spalding,et al.  Effects of background knowledge on category construction , 1996 .

[5]  John D. Coley,et al.  Perspectives on language and thought: Language and categorization: The acquisition of natural kind terms , 1991 .

[6]  M. Gernsbacher,et al.  Proceedings of the 20th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society , 1998 .

[7]  P Bloom,et al.  Theories of artifact categorization. , 1998, Cognition.

[8]  Woo-Kyoung Ahn,et al.  Effect of causal structure on category construction , 1999, Memory & cognition.

[9]  L. Rips Similarity, typicality, and categorization , 1989 .

[10]  Michael R. Waldmann,et al.  Causal models and the acquisition of category structure , 1995 .

[11]  S. Gelman,et al.  Mapping the mind: Essentialist beliefs in children: The acquisition of concepts and theories , 1994 .

[12]  S. Gould,et al.  The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: a critique of the adaptationist programme , 1979, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[13]  S. Stoness,et al.  Proceedings of the 21st Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society , 1999 .

[14]  S. Gelman,et al.  Understanding natural cause: children's explanations of how objects and their properties originate. , 1991, Child development.

[15]  Douglas L Medin,et al.  Linear separability and concept learning: Context, relational properties, and concept naturalness , 1986, Cognitive Psychology.

[16]  B. Rehder A causal-model theory of conceptual representation and categorization. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[17]  T. Judgmen Extrapolating human probability judgment , 1994 .

[18]  B. Ross,et al.  Abstract coherent categories. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[19]  E. Heit,et al.  Similarity and property effects in inductive reasoning. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[20]  R. Nosofsky Attention, similarity, and the identification-categorization relationship. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[21]  Melissa Lin Wu Structure in Category-Based Induction , 2003 .

[22]  Susan A. Gelman,et al.  Inferring Properties from Categories versus Inferring Categories from Properties: The Case of Gender. , 1986 .

[23]  Bob Rehder,et al.  A Causal-Model Theory of Categorization , 1999 .

[24]  W. Ahn Why are different features central for natural kinds and artifacts?: the role of causal status in determining feature centrality , 1998, Cognition.

[25]  D. Krantz,et al.  The use of statistical heuristics in everyday inductive reasoning , 1983 .

[26]  M. Lassaline,et al.  Structural alignment in induction and similarity. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[27]  Mary Utter,et al.  Structural alignment in induction and similarity , 1996 .

[28]  B. Malt,et al.  Category Coherence in Cross-Cultural Perspective , 1995, Cognitive Psychology.

[29]  Steven A. Sloman,et al.  Feature Centrality and Conceptual Coherence , 1998, Cogn. Sci..

[30]  H. Pashler STEVENS' HANDBOOK OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY , 2002 .

[31]  Susan Carey,et al.  Developmental changes within the core of artifact concepts , 2001, Cognition.

[32]  L. Rips Inductive judgments about natural categories. , 1975 .

[33]  J. Hampton Polymorphous Concepts in Semantic Memory , 1979 .

[34]  Douglas L. Medin,et al.  A two-stage model of category construction ☆ , 1992 .

[35]  Paul Thagard,et al.  Variability and confirmation , 1982 .

[36]  D. Medin,et al.  Tall is typical: Central tendency, ideal dimensions, and graded category structure among tree experts and novices , 2000, Memory & cognition.

[37]  D. Medin,et al.  Family resemblance, conceptual cohesiveness, and category construction , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.

[38]  D L Medin,et al.  Expertise and category-based induction. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[39]  Woo-Kyoung Ahn,et al.  A Two-Stage Model of Category Construction , 1992, Cogn. Sci..

[40]  E. Mayr,et al.  Toward a New Philosophy of Biology , 1988 .

[41]  S. Gelman,et al.  Children's inductive inferences within superordinate categories: the role of language and category structure. , 1988, Child development.

[42]  Lawrence A. Hirschfeld,et al.  Race in the Making: Cognition, Culture, and the Child's Construction of Human Kinds , 1996 .

[43]  Douglas L. Medin,et al.  Context theory of classification learning. , 1978 .

[44]  A. Gopnik,et al.  The theory theory. , 1994 .

[45]  A. Ortony,et al.  Similarity and Analogical Reasoning , 1991 .

[46]  L. Barsalou Ideals, central tendency, and frequency of instantiation as determinants of graded structure in categories. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[47]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  The Tree of Life: Universal and Cultural Features of Folkbiological Taxonomies and Inductions , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.

[48]  G. Murphy,et al.  The Big Book of Concepts , 2002 .

[49]  G. Murphy,et al.  The acquisition of category structure in unsupervised learning , 1999, Memory & cognition.

[50]  Wayne D. Gray,et al.  Basic objects in natural categories , 1976, Cognitive Psychology.

[51]  F. Keil Concepts, Kinds, and Cognitive Development , 1989 .

[52]  D. Medin,et al.  The role of theories in conceptual coherence. , 1985, Psychological review.

[53]  Lewis Bott,et al.  Knowledge selection in category learning , 2000 .

[54]  S. Gelman The development of induction within natural kind and artifact categories , 1988, Cognitive Psychology.

[55]  Hilary Kornblith,et al.  Inductive Inference and Its Natural Ground: An Essay in Naturalistic Epistemology , 1993 .

[56]  L. Rips,et al.  Necessity and natural categories. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[57]  N. Goodman Fact, Fiction, and Forecast , 1955 .

[58]  G. Murphy,et al.  Category learning with minimal prior knowledge. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[59]  Lawrence A. Hirschfeld,et al.  Mapping the mind: Toward a topography of mind: An introduction to domain specificity , 1994 .

[60]  Elizabeth F. Shipley,et al.  Categories, hierarchies, and induction , 1993 .

[61]  Paul D. Allopenna,et al.  The locus of knowledge effects in concept learning. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[62]  S. Sloman Feature-Based Induction , 1993, Cognitive Psychology.

[63]  George Lakoff,et al.  Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things , 1987 .

[64]  C. Frith,et al.  Mapping the Mind , 1998 .

[65]  E. Wisniewski,et al.  Prior knowledge and functionally relevant features in concept learning. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[66]  C. Sumiyoshi CATEGORY BASED INDUCTION , 1997 .

[67]  Frank C. Keil,et al.  The growth of causal understandings of natural kinds. , 1995 .

[68]  Paul Bloom Intention, history, and artifact concepts , 1996, Cognition.

[69]  Edward J. Wisniewski,et al.  Concepts and Categorization , 2002 .

[70]  E. Heit Properties of inductive reasoning , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[71]  H. Wellman,et al.  Insides and essences: Early understandings of the non-obvious , 1991, Cognition.

[72]  M. Gluck,et al.  Explaining Basic Categories: Feature Predictability and Information , 1992 .

[73]  R. Hastie,et al.  Causal knowledge and categories: the effects of causal beliefs on categorization, induction, and similarity. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[74]  H. Rachlin Theory-theory theory , 1993, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[75]  E. Rosch,et al.  Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[76]  Bob Rehder,et al.  Categorization as causal reasoning , 2003, Cogn. Sci..

[77]  L. Smith Importance of the overall similarity of objects of adults' and children's classifications. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[78]  R. Brown How shall a thing be called. , 1958, Psychological review.

[79]  J. Maienschein Growth of biological thought , 1994, Nature.

[80]  H. Price Change in View: Principles of Reasoning , 1988 .

[81]  Charles W. Kalish,et al.  Categories and Causality , 1993 .

[82]  Eldar Shafir,et al.  Similarity, plausibility, and judgments of probability , 1993, Cognition.

[83]  G. Murphy,et al.  A knowledge-resonance (KRES) model of category learning , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[84]  D. Medin,et al.  Does rank have its privilege? Inductive inferences within folkbiological taxonomies , 1997, Cognition.