Assessing the Research on Media, Cognitive Development, and Infants

With the rise of television targeting very young children, it is important to ask if babies younger than two can learn from it. In this essay I review the literature on babies' attention to, imitation of, and language learning from television and I discuss the research that has found a video deficit in young children. The literature suggests that infants between the ages of approximately twelve and twenty-four months have a difficult time imitating and learning from television. This deficit may occur because television is perceived by babies as socially irrelevant; it may also occur because television is not as rich in detail as live images. Lastly, it may occur because of the complexity of dual representation. The extant literature also suggests that basic conceptual knowledge is needed in infants before more advanced learning, such as language acquisition, can be achieved. The essay explores whether television is capable of teaching these very basic cognitive skills.

[1]  Rachel Barr,et al.  Age-related changes in deferred imitation from television by 6- to 18-month-olds. , 2007, Developmental science.

[2]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Associations between media viewing and language development in children under age 2 years. , 2007, The Journal of pediatrics.

[3]  Georgene L. Troseth,et al.  Naturalistic Experience and the Early Use of Symbolic Artifacts , 2007 .

[4]  M. Krcmar,et al.  The Development of Aggressive Mental Models in Young Children , 2007 .

[5]  M. Krcmar,et al.  Can Toddlers Learn Vocabulary from Television? An Experimental Approach , 2007 .

[6]  Marie Evans Schmidt,et al.  Two-Year-Olds' Object Retrieval Based on Television: Testing a Perceptual Account , 2007 .

[7]  Rachel Barr,et al.  The effect of repetition on imitation from television during infancy. , 2007, Developmental psychobiology.

[8]  V. Rideout,et al.  Media Family: Electronic Media in the Lives of Infants, Toddlers, Preschoolers, and Their Parents, The , 2006 .

[9]  Georgene L. Troseth,et al.  Young children's use of video as a source of socially relevant information. , 2006, Child development.

[10]  Dimitri A. Christakis,et al.  Teacher in the Living Room? Educational Media for Babies, Toddlers, and Preschoolers, A , 2005 .

[11]  Daniel R. Anderson,et al.  Television and Very Young Children , 2005 .

[12]  Deborah L. Linebarger,et al.  Infants’ and Toddlers’ Television Viewing and Language Outcomes , 2005 .

[13]  Patti M. Valkenburg,et al.  Developmental Changes in Infants’ and Toddlers’ Attention to Television Entertainment , 2004, Commun. Res..

[14]  Georgene L. Troseth,et al.  Getting a clear picture: young children's understanding of a televised image , 2003 .

[15]  H. Hayne,et al.  Imitation from television by 24- and 30-month-olds. , 2003 .

[16]  Daniel R. Anderson,et al.  Television and Reality: Toddlers' Use of Visual Information from Video to Guide Behavior , 2002 .

[17]  E. Thelen Many Roads Lead to Rome: Locomotion and Dynamics. , 2000, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[18]  H. Hayne,et al.  Developmental changes in imitation from television during infancy. , 1999, Child development.

[19]  J. Deloache,et al.  The medium can obscure the message: young children's understanding of video. , 1998, Child development.

[20]  Lori Markson,et al.  Capacities underlying word learning , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[21]  H. Hayne,et al.  Developmental changes in deferred imitation by 6- to 24-month-old infants , 1996 .

[22]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Perception of object unity in young infants: The roles of motion, depth, and orientation , 1996 .

[23]  J. Reznick,et al.  Measuring the vocabulary spurt: a reply to Mervis & Bertrand , 1996, Journal of Child Language.

[24]  A. Meltzoff Understanding the Intentions of Others: Re-Enactment of Intended Acts by 18-Month-Old Children. , 1995, Developmental psychology.

[25]  M. Tomasello First Verbs: A Case Study of Early Grammatical Development , 1994 .

[26]  M L Rice,et al.  Frequency of input effects on word comprehension of children with specific language impairment. , 1994, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[27]  Mabel L. Rice,et al.  Words from Sesame Street : learning vocabulary while viewing , 1990 .

[28]  M L Rice,et al.  Television as a talking picture book: a prop for language acquisition , 1986, Journal of Child Language.

[29]  Elizabeth Pugzles Lorch,et al.  The Effects of TV Program Comprehensibility on Preschool Children's Visual Attention to Television. , 1981 .

[30]  L. Frank The Society for Research in Child Development , 1935 .

[31]  由希 辻 Representation , 2020, The SAGE International Encyclopedia of Mass Media and Society.

[32]  Geraldine Dawson,et al.  Event-related potential (ERP) indices of infants' recognition of familiar and unfamiliar objects in two and three dimensions. , 2006, Developmental science.

[33]  Jean M. Mandler,et al.  How to build a baby III: Image-schemas and the transition to verbal thought. , 2005 .

[34]  Georgene L. Troseth,et al.  From the innocent to the intelligent eye: the early development of pictorial competence. , 2004, Advances in child development and behavior.

[35]  H. Hendershot Nickelodeon nation : the history, politics, and economics of America's only TV channel for kids , 2004 .

[36]  Georgene L. Troseth,et al.  TV guide: two-year-old children learn to use video as a source of information. , 2003, Developmental psychology.

[37]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. , 1998, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[38]  Daniel R. Anderson,et al.  YOUNG CHILDREN'S TV VIEWING: THE PROBLEM OF COGNITIVE CONTINUITY , 1984 .

[39]  R. D. Kavanaugh,et al.  Imitation of live and televised models by children one to three years of age. , 1977, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.