Dissociated stimulus and response conflict effect in the Stroop task: Evidence from evoked brain potentials and brain oscillations
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Men-Tzung Lo,et al. Revealing the brain's adaptability and the transcranial direct current stimulation facilitating effect in inhibitory control by multiscale entropy , 2014, NeuroImage.
[2] Kai Wang,et al. Temporal and spectral profiles of stimulus–stimulus and stimulus–response conflict processing , 2014, NeuroImage.
[3] Robert Oostenveld,et al. Oscillatory dynamics of response competition in human sensorimotor cortex , 2013, NeuroImage.
[4] D. Szűcs,et al. Asymmetry in stimulus and response conflict processing across the adult lifespan: ERP and EMG evidence☆ , 2013, Cortex.
[5] Antao Chen,et al. The neural oscillations of conflict adaptation in the human frontal region , 2013, Biological Psychology.
[6] Zude Zhu,et al. Perceptual conflict-induced late positive complex in a modified Stroop task , 2013, Neuroscience Letters.
[7] Qinglin Zhang,et al. The Neural Dynamics of Conflict Adaptation within a Look-to-Do Transition , 2013, PloS one.
[8] R. Goebel,et al. Local Discriminability Determines the Strength of Holistic Processing for Faces in the Fusiform Face Area , 2013, Front. Psychology.
[9] Ned Jenkinson,et al. A Role for the Subthalamic Nucleus in Response Inhibition during Conflict , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[10] I. David,et al. The Stroop matching task presents conflict at both the response and nonresponse levels: an event-related potential and electromyography study. , 2012, Psychophysiology.
[11] G. Band,et al. Reward valence modulates conflict-driven attentional adaptation: Electrophysiological evidence , 2012, Biological Psychology.
[12] Marcel Bastiaansen,et al. Integration or Predictability? A Further Specification of the Functional Role of Gamma Oscillations in Language Comprehension , 2012, Front. Psychology.
[13] Michael X. Cohen,et al. Theta Dynamics Reveal Domain-specific Control over Stimulus and Response Conflict , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[14] Fruzsina Soltész,et al. Functional definition of the N450 event-related brain potential marker of conflict processing: a numerical stroop study , 2012, BMC Neuroscience.
[15] Mario Liotti,et al. Is conflict monitoring supramodal? Spatiotemporal dynamics of cognitive control processes in an auditory Stroop task , 2012, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[16] R. Nigbur,et al. Theta power as a marker for cognitive interference , 2011, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[17] Thomas V. Wiecki,et al. Subthalamic nucleus stimulation reverses mediofrontal influence over decision threshold , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.
[18] S. Wiens,et al. Behavioral and ERP indices of response conflict in Stroop and flanker tasks. , 2011, Psychophysiology.
[19] Kathy Conklin,et al. Electrophysiological measures of conflict detection and resolution in the Stroop task , 2011, Brain Research.
[20] Antao Chen,et al. Neural correlates of stimulus and response interference in a 2-1 mapping stroop task. , 2011, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[21] Robert Oostenveld,et al. FieldTrip: Open Source Software for Advanced Analysis of MEG, EEG, and Invasive Electrophysiological Data , 2010, Comput. Intell. Neurosci..
[22] Fruzsina Soltész,et al. Stimulus and response conflict in the color–word Stroop task: A combined electro-myography and event-related potential study , 2010, Brain Research.
[23] A. Engel,et al. Beta-band oscillations—signalling the status quo? , 2010, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[24] Dénes Szücs,et al. Motor conflict in Stroop tasks: Direct evidence from single-trial electro-myography and electro-encephalography , 2009, NeuroImage.
[25] Dénes Szücs,et al. Real-time Tracking of Motor Response Activation and Response Competition in a Stroop Task in Young Children: A Lateralized Readiness Potential Study , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[26] John J. B. Allen,et al. Prelude to and Resolution of an Error: EEG Phase Synchrony Reveals Cognitive Control Dynamics during Action Monitoring , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[27] J. Kroger,et al. A functional dissociation of conflict processing within anterior cingulate cortex , 2008, Human brain mapping.
[28] J. Leon Kenemans,et al. Stroop interference and the timing of selective response activation , 2008, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[29] Simon Hanslmayr,et al. The Electrophysiological Dynamics of Interference during the Stroop Task , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[30] Fruzsina Soltész,et al. The interaction of task-relevant and task-irrelevant stimulus features in the number/size congruency paradigm: An ERP study , 2008, Brain Research.
[31] Fruzsina Soltész,et al. Event-related potentials dissociate facilitation and interference effects in the numerical Stroop paradigm , 2007, Neuropsychologia.
[32] R. Oostenveld,et al. Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.
[33] Rajesh P. N. Rao,et al. Spectral Changes in Cortical Surface Potentials during Motor Movement , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[34] Li Yao,et al. Time-Frequency Analysis of EEG Based on Event Related Cognitive Task , 2006, ISNN.
[35] Cameron S. Carter,et al. Separating semantic conflict and response conflict in the Stroop task: A functional MRI study , 2005, NeuroImage.
[36] Nicholas Wymbs,et al. Neural correlates of conflict processing , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.
[37] S. Makeig,et al. Mining event-related brain dynamics , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[38] Matthias Weisbrod,et al. Prefrontal-cingulate activation during executive control: which comes first? , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[39] R. West,et al. Neural correlates of cognitive control and conflict detection in the Stroop and digit-location tasks , 2003, Neuropsychologia.
[40] S. P. Levine,et al. Spatiotemporal patterns of beta desynchronization and gamma synchronization in corticographic data during self-paced movement , 2003, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[41] Jan De Houwer,et al. On the role of stimulus-response and stimulus-stimulus compatibility in the Stroop effect , 2003 .
[42] Thomas E. Nichols,et al. Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: A primer with examples , 2002, Human brain mapping.
[43] M. Botvinick,et al. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.
[44] R. West,et al. Effects of task context and fluctuations of attention on neural activity supporting performance of the Stroop task , 2000, Brain Research.
[45] H. Mayberg,et al. An ERP study of the temporal course of the Stroop color-word interference effect , 2000, Neuropsychologia.
[46] S. Kornblum,et al. A Parallel Distributed Processing Model of Stimulus–Stimulus and Stimulus–Response Compatibility , 1999, Cognitive Psychology.
[47] Colin M. Macleod. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.
[48] M. Coles. Modern mind-brain reading: psychophysiology, physiology, and cognition. , 1989, Psychophysiology.
[49] C. Eriksen,et al. Pre- and poststimulus activation of response channels: a psychophysiological analysis. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[50] Alan S. Brown,et al. Information Processing and Cognition: The Loyola Symposium , 1976 .
[51] F N Dyer,et al. The Stroop phenomenon and its use in the stlldy of perceptual, cognitive, and response processes , 1973, Memory & cognition.
[52] H Egeth,et al. Verbal interference with encoding in a perceptual classification task. , 1970, Journal of experimental psychology.
[53] E. Miller,et al. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.
[54] J. Stroop. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .