Socio-Pragmatics and Attention: Contributions to Gesturally Guided Word Learning in Toddlers

It is clear that gestural cues facilitate early word learning. In hopes of illuminating the relative contributions of attentional and socio-pragmatic factors to the mechanisms by which these cues exert their influence, we taught toddlers novel words with the support of a hierarchy of gestural cues. Twenty-eight- to 31-month-olds heard one of two possible referents labeled with a novel word, while the experimenter gazed at or gazed at and pointed to, touched, or manipulated the target. Learning improved with greater redundancy among cues, with the largest improvement evident when pointing was added to gazing. Looking times revealed that attentional factors accounted for only a small fraction of the variance in performance. Indeed, a significant increase in attention driven by manipulation of the target failed to improve learning. The results therefore suggest a strong role for socio-pragmatic factors in supporting the facilitative effect of gestural cues on word learning.

[1]  Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,et al.  An Emergentist Coalition Model for Word Learning , 2000 .

[2]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Cognitive Psychology: Mind and Brain , 2006 .

[3]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Understanding the link between joint attention and language. , 1995 .

[4]  Cari S. Kilbreath,et al.  Thirteen-month-olds rely on shared labels and shape similarity for inductive inferences. , 2004, Child development.

[5]  M. Tomasello The social bases of language acquisition , 1992 .

[6]  R. Brooks,et al.  Infant Understanding of the Referential Nature of Looking , 2000 .

[7]  L. Gogate,et al.  Intersensory redundancy facilitates learning of arbitrary relations between vowel sounds and objects in seven-month-old infants. , 1998, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[8]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Learning words in nonostensive contexts , 1994 .

[9]  M. Tomasello Perceiving intentions and learning words in the second year of life , 2000 .

[10]  L. Gogate,et al.  The intersensory origins of word‐comprehension: an ecological–dynamic systems view , 2001 .

[11]  A. Woodward Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach , 1998, Cognition.

[12]  C. Moore,et al.  The origins of joint visual attention in infants. , 1998, Developmental psychology.

[13]  Patricia Zukow-Goldring Action to Language via the Mirror Neuron System: Assisted imitation: affordances, effectivities, and the mirror system in early language development , 2006 .

[14]  M. Tomasello,et al.  A new look at infant pointing. , 2007, Child development.

[15]  L. Namy What's in a Name When It Isn't a Word? 17-Month-Olds' Mapping of Nonverbal Symbols to Object Categories. , 2001, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[16]  Beate Sodian,et al.  Preverbal infants' understanding of referential gestures , 2001 .

[17]  Nina C Capone,et al.  Semantic representation and naming in children with specific language impairment. , 2002, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[18]  Roberta Michnick Golinkoff,et al.  Becoming a word learner : a debate on lexical acquisition , 2000 .

[19]  Sandra R. Waxman,et al.  Principles that are invoked in the acquisition of words, but not facts , 2000, Cognition.

[20]  Amanda L. Woodward,et al.  Infants' use of action knowledge to get a grasp on words , 2003 .

[21]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Evidence for ‘motionese’: modifications in mothers’ infant-directed action , 2002 .

[22]  L. Gogate,et al.  Intersensory Redundancy and 7-Month-Old Infants' Memory for Arbitrary Syllable-Object Relations , 2001 .

[23]  Memory in infancy and early childhood. , 2000 .

[24]  E. Markman,et al.  Young children's inductions from natural kinds: the role of categories and appearances. , 1987, Child development.

[25]  J. J. Guajardo,et al.  Infants’ understanding of the point gesture as an object-directed action , 2002 .

[26]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  The development of gaze following and its relation to language. , 2005, Developmental science.

[27]  Larissa K. Samuelson,et al.  Memory and attention make smart word learning: an alternative account of Akhtar, Carpenter, and Tomasello. , 1998, Child development.

[28]  Larissa K. Samuelson,et al.  Fast Mapping but Poor Retention by 24-Month-Old Infants. , 2008, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[29]  Rebecca J. Brand,et al.  Breaking the language barrier: an emergentist coalition model for the origins of word learning. , 2000, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[30]  J. Dannemiller,et al.  Motion-based detection by 14-week-old infants , 1993, Vision Research.

[31]  E. Markman,et al.  Rapid Word Learning in 13- and 18-Month-Olds. , 1994 .

[32]  Patricia J. Bauer,et al.  Holding it all together: How enabling relations facilitate young children's event recall ☆ , 1992 .

[33]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Eighteen-month-old children learn words in non-ostensive contexts , 1996, Journal of Child Language.

[34]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Infants' ability to consult the speaker for clues to word reference , 1993, Journal of Child Language.

[35]  M. Tomasello Joint attention as social cognition. , 1995 .

[36]  S. Waxman,et al.  Object names and object functions serve as cues to categories for infants. , 2002, Developmental psychology.

[37]  A. L. Campbell,et al.  The role of social-referential context in verbal and nonverbal symbol learning. , 2003, Child development.

[38]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  The key is social cognition. , 2003 .

[39]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. , 1998, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[40]  M. Tomasello,et al.  12- and 18-Month-Olds Point to Provide Information for Others , 2006 .

[41]  G. Butterworth,et al.  How the eyes, head and hand serve definite reference , 2000 .

[42]  Jeffrey L. Elman,et al.  Connectionism and language acquisition. , 2001 .

[43]  L. Gogate,et al.  A study of multimodal motherese: the role of temporal synchrony between verbal labels and gestures. , 2000, Child development.

[44]  Shahrzad Mahootian,et al.  The Role of Gesture in Bilingual Education: Does Gesture Enhance Learning? , 2004 .

[45]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Two-year-olds learn words for absent objects and actions. , 1996 .

[46]  Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,et al.  The birth of words: ten-month-olds learn words through perceptual salience. , 2006, Child development.

[47]  M. Casasola,et al.  Acquisition of word-object associations by 14-month-old infants. , 1998, Developmental psychology.

[48]  M. Ricard,et al.  Les relations entre l'attention conjointe au cows de la periode préverbale et le développement de la référence verbale , 1995 .

[49]  Nina C Capone,et al.  The effect of semantic representation on toddlers' word retrieval. , 2005, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[50]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Elaborative encoding as an explanation of levels of processing , 1982 .

[51]  Amanda L. Woodward,et al.  Infants’ developing understanding of the link between looker and object , 2003 .

[52]  C. Moore,et al.  Word learning in the context of referential and salience cues. , 1998, Developmental psychology.

[53]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Teachers’ gestures facilitate students’ learning: A lesson in symmetry , 2003 .

[54]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Twelve-month-olds point to share attention and interest. , 2004, Developmental science.

[55]  The Role of Causality in Encoding and Remembering Events as a Function of Age , 1977 .

[56]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Naming in young children: a dumb attentional mechanism? , 1996, Cognition.

[57]  A. J. Caron,et al.  Comprehension of the Referential Intent of Looking and Pointing Between 12 and 15 Months , 2002 .