Faster processing of moving compared to flashed bars in awake macaque V1 provides a neural correlate of the flash lag illusion

When the brain has determined the position of a moving object, due to anatomical and processing delays, the object will have already moved to a new location. Given the statistical regularities present in natural motion, the brain may have acquired compensatory mechanisms to minimize the mismatch between the perceived and the real position of a moving object. A well-known visual illusion — the flash lag effect — points towards such a possibility. Although many psychophysical models have been suggested to explain this illusion, their predictions have not been tested at the neural level, particularly in a species of animal known to perceive the illusion. Towards this, we recorded neural responses to flashed and moving bars from primary visual cortex (V1) of awake, fixating macaque monkeys. We found that the response latency to moving bars of varying speed, motion direction and luminance was shorter than that to flashes, in a manner that is consistent with psychophysical results. At the level of V1, our results support the differential latency model positing that flashed and moving bars have different latencies. As we found a neural correlate of the illusion in passively fixating monkeys, our results also suggest that judging the instantaneous position of the moving bar at the time of flash — as required by the postdiction/motion-biasing model — may not be necessary for observing a neural correlate of the illusion. Our results also suggest that the brain may have evolved mechanisms to process moving stimuli faster and closer to real time compared with briefly appearing stationary stimuli. New and Noteworthy We report several observations in awake macaque V1 that provide support for the differential latency model of the flash lag illusion. We find that the equal latency of flash and moving stimuli as assumed by motion integration/postdiction models does not hold in V1. We show that in macaque V1, motion processing latency depends on stimulus luminance, speed and motion direction in a manner consistent with several psychophysical properties of the flash lag illusion.

[1]  Gregor Schöner,et al.  Shorter latencies for motion trajectories than for flashes in population responses of cat primary visual cortex , 2004, The Journal of physiology.

[2]  Mazyar Fallah,et al.  A Motion-Dependent Distortion of Retinotopy in Area V4 , 2006, Neuron.

[3]  Harold E Bedell,et al.  Differential latencies and the dynamics of the position computation process for moving targets, assessed with the flash-lag effect , 2004, Vision Research.

[4]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Visual response latencies in striate cortex of the macaque monkey. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  Scott O. Murray,et al.  Object-Centered Shifts of Receptive Field Positions in Monkey Primary Visual Cortex , 2014, Current Biology.

[6]  Preeti Verghese,et al.  Predictability and the Dynamics of Position Processing in the Flash-Lag Effect , 2005, Perception.

[7]  Michael J. Berry,et al.  Anticipation of moving stimuli by the retina , 1999, Nature.

[8]  Markus Lappe,et al.  A model of the perceived relative positions of moving objects based upon a slow averaging process , 2000, Vision Research.

[9]  M. Lappe,et al.  The position of moving objects. , 2000, Perception.

[10]  H. Bedell,et al.  Flash-lag effect: differential latency, not postdiction. , 2000, Science.

[11]  Dale Purves,et al.  An empirical explanation of the flash-lag effect , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[12]  Terrence J Sejnowski,et al.  Motion signals bias localization judgments: a unified explanation for the flash-lag, flash-drag, flash-jump, and Frohlich illusions. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[13]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Illusory spatial offset of a flash relative to a moving stimulus is caused by differential latencies for moving and flashed stimuli , 2000, Vision Research.

[14]  T J Sejnowski,et al.  Motion integration and postdiction in visual awareness. , 2000, Science.

[15]  T. Radil,et al.  Selective directional sensitivity in visual motion perception , 1991, Vision Research.

[16]  S. Klein,et al.  Evidence for an Attentional Component of the Perceptual Misalignment between Moving and Flashing Stimuli , 2002, Perception.

[17]  F A Wichmann,et al.  Ning for Helpful Comments and Suggestions. This Paper Benefited Con- Siderably from Conscientious Peer Review, and We Thank Our Reviewers the Psychometric Function: I. Fitting, Sampling, and Goodness of Fit , 2001 .

[18]  Alexander S. Ecker,et al.  State Dependence of Noise Correlations in Macaque Primary Visual Cortex , 2014, Neuron.

[19]  Rajesh P. N. Rao,et al.  Optimal Smoothing in Visual Motion Perception , 2001, Neural Computation.

[20]  B L McNaughton,et al.  Interpreting neuronal population activity by reconstruction: unified framework with application to hippocampal place cells. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  Felix Wichmann,et al.  The psychometric function: II. Bootstrap-based confidence intervals and sampling , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[22]  Alexander S. Ecker,et al.  Recording chronically from the same neurons in awake, behaving primates. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  D. Mackay Perceptual Stability of a Stroboscopically Lit Visual Field containing Self-Luminous Objects , 1958, Nature.

[24]  G. Rhodes,et al.  Sex-specific norms code face identity. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[25]  Ingo Fründ,et al.  Inference for psychometric functions in the presence of nonstationary behavior. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[26]  Gopathy Purushothaman,et al.  Moving ahead through differential visual latency , 1998, Nature.

[27]  Endel Põder,et al.  Change in feature space is not necessary for the flash-lag effect , 2001, Vision Research.

[28]  T. Sanger,et al.  Probability density estimation for the interpretation of neural population codes. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  Alexander S. Ecker,et al.  Decorrelated Neuronal Firing in Cortical Microcircuits , 2010, Science.

[30]  F. Chavane,et al.  Imaging cortical correlates of illusion in early visual cortex , 2004, Nature.

[31]  Shinsuke Shimojo,et al.  Changing objects lead briefly flashed ones , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[32]  Eric I Knudsen,et al.  Dynamic shifts in the owl's auditory space map predict moving sound location , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[33]  I. Murakami,et al.  Latency difference, not spatial extrapolation , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[34]  Shinichi Kita,et al.  Our own faces: perceiving fluctuating asymmetry in the highly familiar objects , 2010 .

[35]  Stanley A. Klein,et al.  Extrapolation or attention shift? , 1995, Nature.

[36]  Peter Dayan,et al.  Theoretical Neuroscience: Computational and Mathematical Modeling of Neural Systems , 2001 .

[37]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[38]  P Cavanagh,et al.  The Position of Moving Objects , 2000, Science.

[39]  M. Oram Contrast induced changes in response latency depend on stimulus specificity , 2010, Journal of Physiology-Paris.

[40]  B. Richmond,et al.  Latency: another potential code for feature binding in striate cortex. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  Markus Lappe,et al.  Temporal recruitment along the trajectory of moving objects and the perception of position , 1999, Vision Research.

[42]  I. Murakami,et al.  The flash-lag effect as a spatiotemporal correlation structure. , 2001, Journal of vision.

[43]  Denis G. Pelli,et al.  ECVP '07 Abstracts , 2007, Perception.

[44]  F. Mechler,et al.  Temporal coding of contrast in primary visual cortex: when, what, and why. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[45]  M. Kenward,et al.  An Introduction to the Bootstrap , 2007 .

[46]  M. Carandini,et al.  Summation and division by neurons in primate visual cortex. , 1994, Science.

[47]  Romi Nijhawan,et al.  Behavioral significance of motion direction causes anisotropic flash-lag, flash-drag, flash-repulsion, and movement-mislocalization effects. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[48]  Ryota Kanai,et al.  Stopping the motion and sleuthing the flash-lag effect: spatial uncertainty is the key to perceptual mislocalization , 2004, Vision Research.

[49]  Romi Nijhawan,et al.  Motion extrapolation in catching , 1994, Nature.

[50]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[51]  G. Orban,et al.  Velocity selectivity in the cat visual system. I. Responses of LGN cells to moving bar stimuli: a comparison with cortical areas 17 and 18. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[52]  Wei Ji Ma,et al.  Bayesian inference with probabilistic population codes , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[53]  E. Brenner,et al.  Motion extrapolation is not responsible for the flash–lag effect , 2000, Vision Research.

[54]  Alexander S. Ecker,et al.  Macaque Monkeys Perceive the Flash Lag Illusion , 2013, PloS one.

[55]  Robert F. Hess,et al.  Spatial scale invariance of the amblyopic global motion deficit , 2010 .

[56]  Guillaume S. Masson,et al.  The Flash-Lag Effect as a Motion-Based Predictive Shift , 2017, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[57]  Ronald A. Rensink Grouping in visual short-term memory , 2010 .