Developing Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge of Mathematics

Mathematical competence rests on developing knowledge of concepts and of procedures (i.e. conceptual and procedural knowledge). Although there is some variability in how these constructs are defined and measured, there is general consensus that the relations between conceptual and procedural knowledge are often bi-directional and iterative. The chapter reviews recent studies on the relations between conceptual and procedural knowledge in mathematics and highlights examples of instructional methods for supporting both types of knowledge. It concludes with important issues to address in future research, including gathering evidence for the validity of measures of conceptual and procedural knowledge and specifying more comprehensive models for how conceptual and procedural knowledge develop over time.

[1]  Ann Dowker,et al.  Individual differences in arithmetic , 2012, Individual Differences in Arithmetic.

[2]  A. Baroody,et al.  An Alternative Reconceptualization of Procedural and Conceptual Knowledge. , 2007 .

[3]  Nancy C. Lavigne Mutually Informative Measures of Knowledge: Concept Maps Plus Problem Sorts in Statistics , 2005 .

[4]  A. W. Blöte,et al.  Students' flexibility in solving two-digit addition and subtraction problems : Instruction effects , 2001 .

[5]  P. Pattison,et al.  Patterns of knowledge in children's addition. , 2003, Developmental psychology.

[6]  Jeffrey Bisanz,et al.  Use of the mathematical principle of inversion in young children. , 2003, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[7]  G. Halford Children's Understanding: The Development of Mental Models , 1993 .

[8]  K. Crowley,et al.  Constraints On Learning in Nonprivileged Domains , 1994, Cognitive Psychology.

[9]  K. H. Canobi,et al.  Concept-procedure interactions in children's addition and subtraction. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[10]  Bethany Rittle-Johnson,et al.  Iterating between lessons on concepts and procedures can improve mathematics knowledge. , 2009, The British journal of educational psychology.

[11]  M. Alibali,et al.  Transitions in concept acquisition: using the hand to read the mind. , 1993, Psychological review.

[12]  P. Bryant,et al.  Individual differences in conceptual and procedural knowledge when learning fractions. , 2010 .

[13]  Percival G. Matthews,et al.  It Pays to be Organized: Organizing Arithmetic Practice Around Equivalent Values Facilitates Understanding of Math Equivalence , 2012 .

[14]  K. H. Canobi,et al.  The role of number words in preschoolers’ addition concepts and problem‐solving procedures , 2010 .

[15]  R. Glaser Advances in Instructional Psychology , 1978 .

[16]  R. Siegler Emerging Minds: The Process of Change in Children's Thinking , 1996 .

[17]  Michelle Perry,et al.  Learning and Transfer: Instructional Conditions and Conceptual Change , 1991 .

[18]  Martha W. Alibali,et al.  Understanding and Using Principles of Arithmetic: Operations Involving Negative Numbers , 2008, Cogn. Sci..

[19]  Jeffrey Bisanz,et al.  What counts as knowing? The development of conceptual and procedural knowledge of counting from kindergarten through Grade 2. , 2006, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[20]  James Hiebert Missing Links@@@Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge: The Case of Mathematics , 1987 .

[21]  J. A. Dixon,et al.  The representations of the arithmetic operations include functional relationships , 2001, Memory & cognition.

[22]  Peter Bryant,et al.  Individual differences in conceptual and procedural fraction understanding: the role of abilities and school experience. , 2012, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[23]  J. Bisanz,et al.  Developmental change and individual differences in children's multiplication. , 2003, Child development.

[24]  Steven A Hecht,et al.  Sources of Group and Individual Differences in Emerging Fraction Skills. , 2010, Journal of educational psychology.

[25]  Jeffrey C. Shih,et al.  Examining the Quality of Statistical Mathematics Education Research. , 2009 .

[26]  R. Siegler,et al.  Conscious and unconscious strategy discoveries: a microgenetic analysis. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[27]  Michael Schneider,et al.  Relations among conceptual knowledge, procedural knowledge, and procedural flexibility in two samples differing in prior knowledge. , 2011, Developmental psychology.

[28]  Jeffrey Bisanz,et al.  Knowledge of counting principles: how relevant is order irrelevance? , 2010, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[29]  H. Roeyers,et al.  Early markers for arithmetic difficulties , 2007, Educational and Child Psychology.

[30]  Martha W. Alibali,et al.  You'll see what you mean: Students encode equations based on their knowledge of arithmetic , 2004, Cogn. Sci..

[31]  J. Moss Developing Children's Understanding of the Rational Numbers: A New Model and an Experimental Curriculum , 1999 .

[32]  Martha W. Alibali,et al.  Learning Mathematics from Procedural Instruction: Externally Imposed Goals Influence What Is Learned. , 2000 .

[33]  B. Rittle-Johnson,et al.  Flexibility in Problem Solving: The Case of Equation Solving. , 2008 .

[34]  B. Rittle-Johnson,et al.  Developing procedural flexibility: are novices prepared to learn from comparing procedures? , 2012, The British journal of educational psychology.

[35]  A. Karmiloff-Smith Précis of Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[36]  Jane Hurry,et al.  Basic calculation proficiency and mathematics achievement in elementary school children. , 2011 .

[37]  Elida V. Laski,et al.  Is 27 a big number? Correlational and causal connections among numerical categorization, number line estimation, and numerical magnitude comparison. , 2007, Child development.

[38]  Daniel L. Schwartz,et al.  Practicing versus inventing with contrasting cases: The effects of telling first on learning and transfer. , 2011 .

[39]  Carol G. Williams,et al.  Using Concept Maps to Assess Conceptual Knowledge of Function , 1998 .

[40]  James Hiebert,et al.  Instruction, Understanding, and Skill in Multidigit Addition and Subtraction , 1996 .

[41]  K. H. Canobi,et al.  Number words in young children’s conceptual and procedural knowledge of addition, subtraction and inversion , 2008, Cognition.

[42]  J. Anderson,et al.  Acquisition of procedural skills from examples. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[43]  C. Lewis,et al.  Predictors of Early Numeracy: Is There a Place for Mistakes when Learning about Number?. , 2007 .

[44]  Paul Cobb,et al.  Assessment of a problem-centered second-grade mathematics project. , 1991 .

[45]  B. Rittle-Johnson,et al.  Does comparing solution methods facilitate conceptual and procedural knowledge? An experimental study on learning to solve equations. , 2007 .

[46]  Judith Wylie,et al.  Individual differences in trajectories of arithmetical development in typically achieving 5- to 7-year-olds. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[47]  Clarissa A. Thompson,et al.  An integrated theory of whole number and fractions development , 2011, Cognitive Psychology.

[48]  Andrea A. diSessa,et al.  Coherence versus fragmentation in the development of the concept of force , 2004, Cogn. Sci..

[49]  Michael Schneider,et al.  The developmental relations between conceptual and procedural knowledge: a multimethod approach. , 2010, Developmental psychology.

[50]  B. Rittle-Johnson,et al.  The Importance of Prior Knowledge When Comparing Examples: Influences on Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge of Equation Solving , 2009 .

[51]  B. Rittle-Johnson,et al.  Developing Conceptual Understanding and Procedural Skill in Mathematics: An Iterative Process. , 2001 .

[52]  T. P. Carpenter,et al.  Professional development focused on children's algebraic reasoning in elementary school , 2007 .

[53]  Jeremy Kilpatrick,et al.  Adding It Up: Helping Children Learn Mathematics , 2013 .

[54]  Christian Lebiere,et al.  Implicit and explicit learning in ACT-R , 1998 .

[55]  P. Pattison,et al.  The role of conceptual understanding in children's addition problem solving. , 1998, Developmental psychology.

[56]  K. H. Canobi,et al.  Children's profiles of addition and subtraction understanding. , 2005, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[57]  Elsbeth Stern,et al.  The Inverse Relation of Addition and Subtraction: A Knowledge Integration Perspective , 2009 .

[58]  B. Rittle-Johnson,et al.  Conceptual and procedural knowledge of mathematics: Does one lead to the other? , 1999 .

[59]  Andrew Izsák,et al.  "You Have to Count the Squares": Applying Knowledge in Pieces to Learning Rectangular Area , 2005 .

[60]  B. Rittle-Johnson,et al.  The effectiveness of using incorrect examples to support learning about decimal magnitude. , 2012 .

[61]  Ron Sun,et al.  A Bottom-Up Model of Skill Learning , 1998 .

[62]  Niels Taatgen,et al.  Implicit versus explicit : an ACT-R learning perspective 1 Implicit versus explicit : an ACT-R learning perspective Commentary on Dienes & Perner : A theory of implicit and explicit knowledge , 1999 .

[63]  R. Gelman,et al.  Preschoolers' counting: Principles before skill , 1983, Cognition.

[64]  K. Fuson,et al.  Korean children's understanding of multidigit addition and subtraction. , 1992, Child development.

[65]  K. H. Canobi,et al.  Individual Differences in Children's Addition and Subtraction Knowledge. , 2004 .

[66]  Martha W. Alibali,et al.  Learning mathematics from procedural instruction : Externally imposed goals influence what is learned , 2001 .

[67]  Merriam-Webster Merriam-Webster's Collegiate Dictionary , 1998 .

[68]  A. Renkl,et al.  Instructional Aids to Support a Conceptual Understanding of Multiple Representations. , 2009 .

[69]  James P. Byrnes,et al.  Role of Conceptual Knowledge in Mathematical Procedural Learning , 1991 .

[70]  Camilla K. Gilmore,et al.  Patterns of Individual Differences in Conceptual Understanding and Arithmetical Skill: A Meta-Analysis , 2009 .

[71]  J. Star Reconceptualizing procedural knowledge. , 2005 .

[72]  L. Verschaffel,et al.  Conceptualizing, investigating, and enhancing adaptive expertise in elementary mathematics education , 2009 .

[73]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Why practice reduces dual-task interference. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[74]  Kirsten Berthold,et al.  Learning from worked-out examples , 2006 .

[75]  Ann Dowker,et al.  The development of arithmetic concepts and skills: Constructing adaptive expertise , 2003 .

[76]  Steven A. Hecht,et al.  Toward an information processing account of individual differences in fraction skills , 1998 .

[77]  Robert S. Siegler,et al.  The relation between conceptual and procedural knowledge in learning mathematics: A review , 2021, The Development of Mathematical Skills.

[78]  Jon R. Star,et al.  The nature and development of experts’ strategy flexibility for solving equations , 2009 .

[79]  Susan B. Empson,et al.  A longitudinal study of invention and understanding in children's multidigit addition and subtraction , 1998 .

[80]  A. Dowker Individual differences in numerical abilities in preschoolers. , 2008, Developmental science.

[81]  I. Peled,et al.  It's Not Too Late to Conceptualize: Constructing a Generalized Subtraction Schema by Abstracting and Connecting Procedures , 2005 .

[82]  Lenni Haapasalo,et al.  Two Types of Mathematical Knowledge and Their Relation , 2000 .

[83]  Martha W. Alibali,et al.  Does Understanding the Equal Sign Matter? Evidence from Solving Equations , 2006 .

[84]  J. Hiebert,et al.  Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge in Mathematics: An Introductory Analysis , 1986 .

[85]  Julie L. Booth,et al.  Development of numerical estimation in young children. , 2004, Child development.

[86]  Steven A Hecht,et al.  Sources of individual differences in fraction skills. , 2003, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[87]  B. Rittle-Johnson,et al.  Compared With What? The Effects of Different Comparisons on Conceptual Knowledge and Procedural Flexibility for Equation Solving , 2009 .

[88]  Lauren B. Resnick Syntax and Semantics in Learning to Subtract. , 1982 .

[89]  Percival G. Matthews,et al.  In pursuit of knowledge: comparing self-explanations, concepts, and procedures as pedagogical tools. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[90]  H. Mandl,et al.  Learning from Worked-Out Examples: The Effects of Example Variability and Elicited Self-Explanations , 1998, Contemporary educational psychology.

[91]  Mark Fox Educational and child psychology , 2013 .

[92]  Preschool Children's Judgments of Number Magnitude. , 1988 .

[93]  David E. Kieras,et al.  VIRTUALLY PERFECT TIME SHARING IN DUAL-TASK PERFORMANCE: , 2001 .

[94]  Dorothea P. Simon,et al.  Expert and Novice Performance in Solving Physics Problems , 1980, Science.

[95]  B. Rittle-Johnson,et al.  Promoting transfer: effects of self-explanation and direct instruction. , 2006, Child development.

[96]  Michael Schneider,et al.  Mental number line, number line estimation, and mathematical achievement : Their interrelations in grades 5 and 6 , 2009 .

[97]  Herbert P. Ginsburg,et al.  Entering the child's mind , 1997 .

[98]  Patricia S. Moyer,et al.  Third Graders Learn about Fractions Using Virtual Manipulatives: A Classroom study , 2005 .

[99]  B. Rittle-Johnson,et al.  It pays to compare: an experimental study on computational estimation. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[100]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Rules of the Mind , 1993 .

[101]  S. Vosniadou,et al.  Understanding the structure of the set of rational numbers: a conceptual change approach , 2004 .

[102]  Rochel Gelman,et al.  Enabling constraints for cognitive development and learning: Domain specificity and epigenesis. , 1998 .

[103]  Jennifer B. Esterly,et al.  Coherence versus fragmentation in the development of the concept of force , 2004 .