Enhancing voluntary imitation through attention and motor imagery

Action observation activates brain areas involved in performing the same action and has been shown to increase motor learning, with potential implications for neurorehabilitation. Recent work indicates that the effects of action observation on movement can be increased by motor imagery or by directing attention to observed actions. In voluntary imitation, activation of the motor system during action observation is already increased. We therefore explored whether imitation could be further enhanced by imagery or attention. Healthy participants observed and then immediately imitated videos of human hand movement sequences, while movement kinematics were recorded. Two blocks of trials were completed, and after the first block participants were instructed to imagine performing the observed movement (Imagery group, N = 18) or attend closely to the characteristics of the movement (Attention group, N = 15), or received no further instructions (Control group, N = 17). Kinematics of the imitated movements were modulated by instructions, with both Imagery and Attention groups being closer in duration, peak velocity and amplitude to the observed model compared with controls. These findings show that both attention and motor imagery can increase the accuracy of imitation and have implications for motor learning and rehabilitation. Future work is required to understand the mechanisms by which these two strategies influence imitation accuracy.

[1]  M. Smyth,et al.  Movement imagery in rock climbing: patterns of interference from visual, spatial and kinaesthetic secondary tasks , 1998 .

[2]  I. Robertson,et al.  Measuring motor imagery ability: A review , 2008 .

[3]  Emily S. Cross,et al.  Sensitivity of the action observation network to physical and observational learning. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[4]  Stefan Vogt,et al.  Automatic Imitation in Rhythmical Actions: Kinematic Fidelity and the Effects of Compatibility, Delay, and Visual Monitoring , 2012, PloS one.

[5]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Irritability in pre-clinical Huntington's disease , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Exploring the functional and anatomical bases of mirror-image and anatomical imitation: The role of the frontal lobes , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  Digby Elliott,et al.  Top-down attentional processes modulate the coding of atypical biological motion kinematics in the absence of motor signals. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[9]  S. Small,et al.  The mirror neuron system and treatment of stroke. , 2012, Developmental psychobiology.

[10]  Francine Malouin,et al.  Towards the integration of mental practice in rehabilitation programs. A critical review , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[11]  S. Small,et al.  Functions of the Mirror Neuron System: Implications for Neurorehabilitation , 2006, Cognitive and behavioral neurology : official journal of the Society for Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology.

[12]  Jungseo Park,et al.  Drinking Behavior Training for Stroke Patients Using Action Observation and Practice of Upper Limb Function , 2013, Journal of physical therapy science.

[13]  M. Brass,et al.  Hyperimitation of Actions Is Related to Reduced Understanding of Others' Minds in Autism Spectrum Conditions , 2010, Biological Psychiatry.

[14]  F. Tremblay,et al.  Differential modulation of corticospinal excitability during observation, mental imagery and imitation of hand actions , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  Cornelius Weiller,et al.  Neural bases of imitation and pantomime in acute stroke patients: distinct streams for praxis. , 2014, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[16]  L. Cohen,et al.  Concurrent action observation modulates practice‐induced motor memory formation , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.

[17]  G. Buccino,et al.  Improving upper limb motor functions through action observation treatment: a pilot study in children with cerebral palsy , 2012, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[18]  J. Decety,et al.  Neural mechanisms subserving the perception of human actions , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[19]  Aymeric Guillot,et al.  Multiple roles of motor imagery during action observation , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[20]  Stephen A Coombes,et al.  Mechanisms of attentional cueing during observational learning to facilitate motor skill acquisition , 2003, Journal of sports sciences.

[21]  G. Humphreys,et al.  The role of the basal ganglia in action imitation: neuropsychological evidence from Parkinson’s disease patients , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  C. Richards,et al.  Brain activity during visual versus kinesthetic imagery: An fMRI study , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[23]  Steven P. Tipper,et al.  Focusing on body sites: the role of spatial attention in action perception , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  What imitation tells us about social cognition: a rapprochement between developmental psychology and cognitive neuroscience. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[25]  T. Muraoka,et al.  Modulation of Corticospinal Excitability during Acquisition of Action Sequences by Observation , 2012, PloS one.

[26]  M. Jeannerod The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[27]  E. Poliakoff,et al.  The influence of goals on movement kinematics during imitation , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  F. Tremblay,et al.  Corticomotor facilitation associated with observation and imagery of hand actions is impaired in Parkinson’s disease , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  E. Zohary,et al.  Mirror-image representation of action in the anterior parietal cortex , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[30]  J. Mattingley,et al.  The role of selective attention in matching observed and executed actions , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[31]  W. Helsen,et al.  Keeping an eye on imagery: the role of eye movements during motor imagery training , 2011, Neuroscience.

[32]  Arnaud Badets,et al.  Intention in motor learning through observation , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[33]  Antonia F. de C. Hamilton,et al.  Social top-down response modulation (STORM): a model of the control of mimicry in social interaction , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[34]  E Poliakoff,et al.  How does visuomotor priming differ for biological and non-biological stimuli? A review of the evidence , 2012, Psychological Research.

[35]  Francine Malouin,et al.  Is somatosensory excitability more affected by the perspective or modality content of motor imagery? , 2011, Neuroscience Letters.

[36]  William S Anderson,et al.  Review of motor and phantom-related imagery , 2011, Neuroreport.

[37]  Emma Gowen,et al.  Imitation in autism: why action kinematics matter , 2012, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[38]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. , 1999, Science.

[39]  C. Heyes Automatic imitation. , 2011, Psychological bulletin.

[40]  Timothy D. Lee,et al.  Motor Control and Learning: A Behavioral Emphasis , 1982 .

[41]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[42]  D. Kiper,et al.  Virtual Reality–Augmented Neurorehabilitation Improves Motor Function and Reduces Neuropathic Pain in Patients With Incomplete Spinal Cord Injury , 2013, Neurorehabilitation and neural repair.

[43]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neural Circuits Underlying Imitation Learning of Hand Actions An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2004, Neuron.

[44]  P. Jolicoeur,et al.  A Solution to the Effect of Sample Size on Outlier Elimination , 1994 .

[45]  Maarten J. IJzerman,et al.  Differential cortical activation during observation and observation-and-imagination , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.

[46]  C. Bradshaw,et al.  Exploring visuomotor priming following biological and non-biological stimuli , 2010, Brain and Cognition.

[47]  C. Marsden,et al.  Apraxia in Parkinson's disease, progressive supranuclear palsy, multiple system atrophy and neuroleptic-induced parkinsonism. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[48]  W. Prinz,et al.  Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. , 2001, Acta psychologica.

[49]  G. Buccino,et al.  Action observation treatment improves autonomy in daily activities in Parkinson's disease patients: Results from a pilot study , 2011, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[50]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[51]  S. Vogt,et al.  Visuomotor priming by pictures of hand postures: perspective matters , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[52]  E. Poliakoff,et al.  Goal-Directed and Goal-Less Imitation in Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2011, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.

[53]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Grasping the Intentions of Others with One's Own Mirror Neuron System , 2005, PLoS biology.

[54]  Jason B. Mattingley,et al.  Selective attention modulates inferior frontal gyrus activity during action observation , 2008, NeuroImage.

[55]  Stefan Vogt,et al.  Motor imagery during action observation modulates automatic imitation effects in rhythmical actions , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[56]  T. Chartrand,et al.  The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[57]  W. Helsen,et al.  The eyes as a mirror of our thoughts: Quantification of motor imagery of goal-directed movements through eye movement registration , 2008, Behavioural Brain Research.

[58]  M. Schoenfeld,et al.  Action Imagery Combined With Action Observation Activates More Corticomotor Regions Than Action Observation Alone , 2012, Journal of neurologic physical therapy : JNPT.

[59]  Kristen L. Macuga,et al.  Neural representations involved in observed, imagined, and imitated actions are dissociable and hierarchically organized , 2012, NeuroImage.