Baby FaceTime: can toddlers learn from online video chat?

There is abundant evidence for the 'video deficit': children under 2 years old learn better in person than from video. We evaluated whether these findings applied to video chat by testing whether children aged 12-25 months could form relationships with and learn from on-screen partners. We manipulated social contingency: children experienced either real-time FaceTime conversations or pre-recorded Videos as the partner taught novel words, actions and patterns. Children were attentive and responsive in both conditions, but only children in the FaceTime group responded to the partner in a temporally synced manner. After one week, children in the FaceTime condition (but not the Video condition) preferred and recognized their Partner, learned more novel patterns, and the oldest children learned more novel words. Results extend previous studies to demonstrate that children under 2 years show social and cognitive learning from video chat because it retains social contingency. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at: https://youtu.be/rTXaAYd5adA.

[1]  Rebekah A. Richert,et al.  Media as social partners: the social nature of young children's learning from screen media. , 2011, Child development.

[2]  G. Troseth,et al.  TV guide: two-year-old children learn to use video as a source of information. , 2003, Developmental psychology.

[3]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Altruistic Helping in Human Infants and Young Chimpanzees , 2006, Science.

[4]  Rebekah A. Richert,et al.  Just a talking book? Word learning from watching baby videos. , 2009, The British journal of developmental psychology.

[5]  Georgene L. Troseth,et al.  Is it life or is it Memorex? Video as a representation of reality , 2010 .

[6]  Georgene L. Troseth,et al.  Third-party social interaction and word learning from video. , 2011, Child development.

[7]  Daniel R. Anderson Educational Television is not an Oxymoron , 1998 .

[8]  H. Hayne,et al.  Imitation from television by 24- and 30-month-olds. , 2003 .

[9]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. , 1998, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[10]  Georgene L. Troseth,et al.  Young children's use of video as a source of socially relevant information. , 2006, Child development.

[11]  Alexis R. Lauricella,et al.  Contingent computer interactions for young children's object retrieval success , 2010 .

[12]  Georgene L. Troseth,et al.  Video Versus Reality: Infants' Attention and Affective Responses to Video and Live Presentations , 2008 .

[13]  Gabrielle A. Strouse,et al.  Can Babies Learn to Read? A Randomized Trial of Baby Media , 2014 .

[14]  Daniel R. Anderson,et al.  Television and Reality: Toddlers' Use of Visual Information from Video to Guide Behavior , 2002 .

[15]  P. Kuhl,et al.  Foreign-language experience in infancy: Effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[16]  M. Blades,et al.  Young people and the media: special issue. , 2009, The British journal of developmental psychology.

[17]  M. Krcmar,et al.  Can Toddlers Learn Vocabulary from Television? An Experimental Approach , 2007 .

[18]  Catharine H. Echols,et al.  The influence of speaker reliability on first versus second label learning. , 2012, Child development.

[19]  Rachel Barr,et al.  Age-related changes in deferred imitation from television by 6- to 18-month-olds. , 2007, Developmental science.

[20]  Rachel Barr,et al.  Transfer of learning between 2D and 3D sources during infancy: Informing theory and practice. , 2010, Developmental review : DR.

[21]  Daniel R. Anderson,et al.  Television and Very Young Children , 2005 .

[22]  Joanne Tarasuik,et al.  Seeing is Believing but is Hearing? Comparing Audio and Video Communication for Young Children , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[23]  Dimitri A Christakis,et al.  Interactive media use at younger than the age of 2 years: time to rethink the American Academy of Pediatrics guideline? , 2014, JAMA pediatrics.

[24]  L. Namy What's in a Name When It Isn't a Word? 17-Month-Olds' Mapping of Nonverbal Symbols to Object Categories. , 2001, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[25]  Carla Kemp Special clothing no better than regular garments for sun protection , 2015, AAP News.

[26]  S. Birch,et al.  The effects of contingency in previous interactions on infants’ preference for social partners , 1999 .

[27]  Gabrielle A. Strouse,et al.  "Don't try this at home": toddlers' imitation of new skills from people on video. , 2008, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[28]  Brittany Huber,et al.  Young children's transfer of learning from a touchscreen device , 2016, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[29]  A. Meltzoff Infant Imitation After a 1-Week Delay: Long-Term Memory for Novel Acts and Multiple Stimuli. , 1988, Developmental psychology.

[30]  Georgene L. Troseth,et al.  Do Babies Learn From Baby Media? , 2010, Psychological science.

[31]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Links between Social Understanding and Early Word Learning: Challenges to Current Accounts. , 2001 .

[32]  L. Namy,et al.  Infants learn baby signs from video. , 2015, Child development.

[33]  S. Waxman,et al.  Early word-learning entails reference, not merely associations , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[34]  J. Deloache,et al.  The medium can obscure the message: young children's understanding of video. , 1998, Child development.

[35]  M. Tomasello Joint attention as social cognition. , 1995 .

[36]  A. Meltzoff Understanding the Intentions of Others: Re-Enactment of Intended Acts by 18-Month-Old Children. , 1995, Developmental psychology.

[37]  Heather L. Kirkorian,et al.  Toddlers' Word Learning From Contingent and Noncontingent Video on Touch Screens. , 2016, Child development.

[38]  Alicia F. Lieberman,et al.  Early Face-to-Face Interaction and Its Relation to Later Infant-Mother Attachment. , 1977 .

[39]  Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,et al.  Putting Education in “Educational” Apps , 2015, Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society.

[40]  Marina Krcmar,et al.  Can Social Meaningfulness and Repeat Exposure Help Infants and Toddlers Overcome the Video Deficit? , 2010 .

[41]  Joanne Tarasuik,et al.  Almost Being There: Video Communication with Young Children , 2011, PloS one.

[42]  Ari Brown Media Use by Children Younger Than 2 Years , 2011, Pediatrics.

[43]  H. Hayne,et al.  Developmental changes in imitation from television during infancy. , 1999, Child development.

[44]  Marina Krcmar,et al.  Word Learning in Very Young Children From Infant‐Directed DVDs , 2011 .

[45]  G. Simcock,et al.  The effect of social engagement on 24-month-olds' imitation from live and televised models. , 2008, Developmental science.

[46]  I. Bretherton The Origins of Attachment Theory: John Bowlby and Mary Ainsworth , 1992 .

[47]  Georgene L. Troseth,et al.  Supporting toddlers’ transfer of word learning from video , 2014 .

[48]  Sarah E. Vaala,et al.  Screen media and language development in infants and toddlers: An ecological perspective , 2010 .

[49]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Infant Imitation from Television Using Noveltouch Screen Technology , 2022 .

[50]  R. Barr Memory Constraints on Infant Learning From Picture Books, Television, and Touchscreens , 2013 .

[51]  Ari Brown,et al.  Beyond ‘turn it off’: How to advise families on media use , 2015, AAP News.

[52]  Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,et al.  Skype me! Socially contingent interactions help toddlers learn language. , 2014, Child development.

[53]  Elisabeth McClure,et al.  "Facetime doesn't count": Video chat as an exception to media restrictions for infants and toddlers , 2015, Int. J. Child Comput. Interact..

[54]  K. Hirsh-Pasek,et al.  Live action: can young children learn verbs from video? , 2009, Child development.

[55]  Elaine Reese,et al.  Picture book reading with young children: A conceptual framework , 2005 .

[56]  A. Bigelow,et al.  Sensitivity to Social Contingency From Mothers and Strangers in 2‐, 4‐, and 6‐Month‐Old Infants , 2003 .

[57]  S. Carey,et al.  Contingency is not enough: Social context guides third-party attributions of intentional agency. , 2014, Developmental psychology.

[58]  Harlene Hayne,et al.  Memory retrieval by 18--30-month-olds: age-related changes in representational flexibility. , 2000 .

[59]  A. Woodward,et al.  Sensitivity of 24-month-olds to the prior inaccuracy of the source: possible mechanisms. , 2010, Developmental psychology.

[60]  Sylvia M. J. Hains,et al.  Effects of stimulus contingency in infant-adult interactions☆ , 1996 .

[61]  A. L. Campbell,et al.  The role of social-referential context in verbal and nonverbal symbol learning. , 2003, Child development.