Temporal stimulus properties that attract gaze to the periphery and repel gaze from fixation.

Humans use saccadic eye movements to fixate different parts of their visual environment. While stimulus features that determine the location of the next fixation in static images have been extensively studied, temporal stimulus features that determine the timing of the gaze shifts received less attention. It is also unclear if stimulus features at the present gaze location can trigger gaze shifts to another location. To investigate these questions, we asked observers to switch their gaze between two blobs. In three different conditions, either the fixated blob, the peripheral blob, or both blobs were flickering. A time-frequency analysis of the flickering noise values, time locked to the gaze shifts, revealed significant phase locking in a time window 300 to 100 ms before the gaze shift at temporal frequencies below 20 Hz. The average phase angles at these time-frequency points indicated that observer's gaze was repelled by decreasing contrast of the fixated blob and attracted by increasing contrast of the peripheral blob. These results show that temporal properties of both, fixated, and peripheral stimuli are capable of triggering gaze shifts.

[1]  D. Munoz,et al.  Neuronal Activity in Monkey Superior Colliculus Related to the Initiation of Saccadic Eye Movements , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  Alexander C. Schütz,et al.  Eye movements and perception: a selective review. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[3]  Alexander C. Schütz,et al.  Dynamic integration of information about salience and value for saccadic eye movements , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[4]  M. Land Eye movements and the control of actions in everyday life , 2006, Progress in Retinal and Eye Research.

[5]  A Mouraux,et al.  Across-trial averaging of event-related EEG responses and beyond. , 2008, Magnetic resonance imaging.

[6]  G. Folland,et al.  The uncertainty principle: A mathematical survey , 1997 .

[7]  Miguel P Eckstein,et al.  Classification image analysis: estimation and statistical inference for two-alternative forced-choice experiments. , 2002, Journal of vision.

[8]  A. Bovik,et al.  Visual search in noise: revealing the influence of structural cues by gaze-contingent classification image analysis. , 2006, Journal of vision.

[9]  Robert M. McPeek,et al.  Superior colliculus activity related to concurrent processing of saccade goals in a visual search task. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  Eugene McSorley,et al.  The influence of spatial frequency and contrast on saccade latencies , 2004, Vision Research.

[11]  Peter Neri,et al.  Nonlinear characterization of a simple process in human vision. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[12]  W. Geisler Visual perception and the statistical properties of natural scenes. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[13]  Miguel P Eckstein,et al.  The time course of visual information accrual guiding eye movement decisions. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[14]  C. Koch,et al.  Computational modelling of visual attention , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[15]  H. Brettel,et al.  The peripheral critical flicker frequency , 1979, Vision Research.

[16]  D. Kerzel,et al.  Influence of target and distractor contrast on the remote distractor effect , 2008, Vision Research.

[17]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[18]  R. Hess,et al.  Temporal frequency filters in the human peripheral visual field , 1992, Vision Research.

[19]  D. Munoz,et al.  A neural correlate for the gap effect on saccadic reaction times in monkey. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  C. J. Erkelens,et al.  Control of fixation duration in a simple search task , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.

[21]  J. Pernier,et al.  Stimulus Specificity of Phase-Locked and Non-Phase-Locked 40 Hz Visual Responses in Human , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  P. Perona,et al.  Objects predict fixations better than early saliency. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[23]  John M. Henderson,et al.  Attentional selection in scene viewing is object-based , 2010 .

[24]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[25]  David J. Heeger,et al.  Spatiotemporal mechanisms for detecting and identifying image features in human vision , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[26]  M. Saslow Effects of components of displacement-step stimuli upon latency for saccadic eye movement. , 1967, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[27]  R. VanRullen,et al.  The phase of ongoing EEG oscillations predicts visual perception , 2010 .

[28]  R. Klein,et al.  A Model of Saccade Initiation Based on the Competitive Integration of Exogenous and Endogenous Signals in the Superior Colliculus , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[29]  D. Ballard,et al.  Eye guidance in natural vision: reinterpreting salience. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[30]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[31]  D. H. Kelly Motion and vision. II. Stabilized spatio-temporal threshold surface. , 1979, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[32]  G. Loftus Picture perception: effects of luminance on available information and information-extraction rate. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[33]  Karl R Gegenfurtner,et al.  Visual orienting in dynamic broadband (1/f) noise sequences , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[34]  H. Deubel,et al.  Effect of remote distractors on saccade programming: evidence for an extended fixation zone. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  S. Makeig,et al.  Mining event-related brain dynamics , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[36]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  Computational models of cortical visual processing. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[37]  C. Koch,et al.  Faces and text attract gaze independent of the task: Experimental data and computer model. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[38]  A. Nobre,et al.  The hazards of time , 2007, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[39]  Thomas Martinetz,et al.  Variability of eye movements when viewing dynamic natural scenes. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[40]  R. VanRullen,et al.  This Is the Rhythm of Your Eyes: The Phase of Ongoing Electroencephalogram Oscillations Modulates Saccadic Reaction Time , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  D. E. Irwin,et al.  Our Eyes do Not Always Go Where we Want Them to Go: Capture of the Eyes by New Objects , 1998 .

[42]  Mriganka Sur,et al.  Image Structure at the Center of Gaze during Free Viewing , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[43]  I. Rentschler,et al.  Peripheral vision and pattern recognition: a review. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[44]  Casimir J. H. Ludwig,et al.  The Temporal Impulse Response Underlying Saccadic Decisions , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Tracking the mind during reading: the influence of past, present, and future words on fixation durations. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[46]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  A visual salience map in the primate frontal eye field. , 2005, Progress in brain research.

[47]  J. Atick,et al.  STATISTICS OF NATURAL TIME-VARYING IMAGES , 1995 .

[48]  D. Mathalon,et al.  Event-related EEG time-frequency analysis: an overview of measures and an analysis of early gamma band phase locking in schizophrenia. , 2008, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[49]  John M. Henderson,et al.  Clustering of Gaze During Dynamic Scene Viewing is Predicted by Motion , 2011, Cognitive Computation.

[50]  P. Neri Estimation of nonlinear psychophysical kernels. , 2004, Journal of vision.

[51]  Frans W Cornelissen,et al.  The Eyelink Toolbox: Eye tracking with MATLAB and the Psychophysics Toolbox , 2002, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[52]  Jillian H. Fecteau,et al.  Vying for dominance: dynamic interactions control visual fixation and saccadic initiation in the superior colliculus. , 2002, Progress in brain research.

[53]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Saccades, salience and attention: the role of the lateral intraparietal area in visual behavior. , 2006, Progress in brain research.

[54]  Richard F Murray,et al.  Classification images: A review. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[55]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: A primer with examples , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[56]  Miguel P Eckstein,et al.  Saccadic and perceptual performance in visual search tasks. I. Contrast detection and discrimination. , 2003, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[57]  J. Findlay,et al.  Active Vision: The Psychology of Looking and Seeing , 2003 .

[58]  Zhaoping Li A saliency map in primary visual cortex , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[59]  Karl R. Gegenfurtner,et al.  The contribution of low-level features at the centre of gaze to saccade target selection , 2009, Vision Research.

[60]  J. Gallant,et al.  Goal-Related Activity in V4 during Free Viewing Visual Search Evidence for a Ventral Stream Visual Salience Map , 2003, Neuron.

[61]  Miguel P Eckstein,et al.  Visual search: a retrospective. , 2011, Journal of vision.