Navigation outside of the box: what the lab can learn from the field and what the field can learn from the lab

Space is continuous. But the communities of researchers that study the cognitive map in non-humans are strangely divided, with debate over its existence found among behaviorists but not neuroscientists. To reconcile this and other debates within the field of navigation, we return to the concept of the parallel map theory, derived from data on hippocampal function in laboratory rodents. Here the cognitive map is redefined as the integrated map, which is a construction of dual mechanisms, one based on directional cues (bearing map) and the other on positional cues (sketch map). We propose that the dual navigational mechanisms of pigeons, the navigational map and the familiar area map, could be homologous to these mammalian parallel maps; this has implications for both research paradigms. Moreover, this has implications for the lab. To create a bearing map (and hence integrated map) from extended cues requires self-movement over a large enough space to sample and model these cues at a high resolution. Thus a navigator must be able to move freely to map extended cues; only then should the weighted hierarchy of available navigation mechanisms shift in favor of the integrated map. Because of the paucity of extended cues in the lab, the flexible solutions allowed by the integrated map should be rare, despite abundant neurophysiological evidence for the existence of the machinery needed to encode and map extended cues through voluntary movement. Not only do animals need to map extended cues but they must also have sufficient information processing capacity. This may require a specific ontogeny, in which the navigator’s nervous system is exposed to naturally complex spatial contingencies, a circumstance that occurs rarely, if ever, in the lab. For example, free-ranging, flying animals must process more extended cues than walking animals and for this reason alone, the integrated map strategy may be found more reliably in some species. By taking concepts from ethology and the parallel map theory, we propose a path to directly integrating the three great experimental paradigms of navigation: the honeybee, the homing pigeon and the laboratory rodent, towards the goal of a robust, unified theory of animal navigation.

[1]  G L Miklos,et al.  Molecules and cognition: the latterday lessons of levels, language, and lac. Evolutionary overview of brain structure and function in some vertebrates and invertebrates. , 1993, Journal of neurobiology.

[2]  V P Bingman,et al.  Hippocampal lesions impair navigational learning in experienced homing pigeons. , 1992, Behavioral neuroscience.

[3]  Bruce L. McNaughton,et al.  Path integration and the neural basis of the 'cognitive map' , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[4]  R. Menzel,et al.  Development and experience lead to increased volume of subcompartments of the honeybee mushroom body. , 1994, Behavioral and neural biology.

[5]  Wolfgang Wiltschko,et al.  Clock-shift experiments with homing pigeons: a compromise between solar and magnetic information? , 2001, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[6]  P. Hoogland,et al.  Medial cortex of the lizard Gekko gecko: A hodological study with emphasis on regional specialization , 1993, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[7]  Gerhard Tröster,et al.  Pigeon Homing along Highways and Exits , 2004, Current Biology.

[8]  T. S. Collett,et al.  Landmark learning in bees , 1983, Journal of comparative physiology.

[9]  R. Muheim,et al.  Magnetic Maps in Animals: A Theory Comes of Age? , 2006, The Quarterly Review of Biology.

[10]  Lucia F. Jacobs,et al.  The Evolution of the Cognitive Map , 2003, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.

[11]  J. Gerstner,et al.  Circadian rhythms and memory formation , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[12]  V P Bingman,et al.  Homing behavior of pigeons after telencephalic ablations. , 1984, Brain, behavior and evolution.

[13]  B. Crespi,et al.  The adaptive significance of adult neurogenesis: an integrative approach , 2013, Front. Neuroanat..

[14]  Kenneth P. Able,et al.  The concepts and terminology of bird navigation , 2001 .

[15]  Erich D Jarvis,et al.  Evolution of the Pallium in Birds and Reptiles , 2022 .

[16]  Gerald E. Hough,et al.  The Avian Hippocampus, Homing in Pigeons and the Memory Representation of Large-Scale Space1 , 2005, Integrative and comparative biology.

[17]  Lucia F. Jacobs,et al.  The Role of Social Selection in the Evolution of Hippocampal Specialization , 2009 .

[18]  Nachum Ulanovsky,et al.  Large-scale navigational map in a mammal , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[19]  F. Dyer Bees acquire route-based memories but not cognitive maps in a familiar landscape , 1991, Animal Behaviour.

[20]  Eric Hardy.,et al.  Bird Navigation , 1971, Nature.

[21]  William H Warren,et al.  Do humans integrate routes into a cognitive map? Map- versus landmark-based navigation of novel shortcuts. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[22]  Thom Herrmann,et al.  Spatial problem solving by rats: Exploration and cognitive maps , 1982 .

[23]  R. Menzel,et al.  Do insects have cognitive maps? , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[25]  N. Maier A study of orientation in the rat. , 1932 .

[26]  Hanspeter A. Mallot,et al.  Animal navigation: A synthesis , 2011 .

[27]  Denise Manahan-Vaughan,et al.  Learning-Facilitated Synaptic Plasticity at CA3 Mossy Fiber and Commissural–Associational Synapses Reveals Different Roles in Information Processing , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[28]  G. Kramer,et al.  EXPERIMENTS ON BIRD ORIENTATION , 2008 .

[29]  Verner P Bingman,et al.  Hippocampal‐dependent familiar area map supports corrective re‐orientation following navigational error during pigeon homing: a GPS‐tracking study , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.

[30]  Lucia F. Jacobs,et al.  From chemotaxis to the cognitive map: The function of olfaction , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[31]  J. L. Gittleman,et al.  Carnivore olfactory bulb size allometry phylogeny and ecology , 1991 .

[32]  Lucia F. Jacobs,et al.  Sex differences in memory for landmark arrays in C57BL/J6 mice , 2013, Animal Cognition.

[33]  Neil Burgess,et al.  Space for the brain in the cognitive science , 2009 .

[34]  William A Roberts,et al.  Rats take correct novel routes and shortcuts in an enclosed maze. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[35]  J. Wild,et al.  Fiber connections of the hippocampal formation and septum and subdivisions of the hippocampal formation in the pigeon as revealed by tract tracing and kainic acid lesions , 2004, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[36]  T. Robinson,et al.  Brain Plasticity and Behavior , 2003, Annual review of psychology.

[37]  V. Bingman,et al.  The avian hippocampus: evidence for a role in the development of the homing pigeon navigational map. , 1990, Behavioral neuroscience.

[38]  Norman R. F. Maier,et al.  The effect of cerebral destruction on reasoning and learning in rats , 1932 .

[39]  Sandeep Gupta,et al.  Defining structural homology between the mammalian and avian hippocampus through conserved gene expression patterns observed in the chick embryo. , 2012, Developmental biology.

[40]  E. Tolman,et al.  Studies in spatial learning: Orientation and the short-cut. , 1946, Journal of experimental psychology.

[41]  Valeria Anna Sovrano,et al.  How fish do geometry in large and in small spaces , 2006, Animal Cognition.

[42]  G. Robinson,et al.  Experience-expectant plasticity in the mushroom bodies of the honeybee. , 1998, Learning & memory.

[43]  Dr. Roswitha Wiltschko,et al.  Magnetic Orientation in Animals , 1995, Zoophysiology.

[44]  F. Huntingford Animal Thinking, Donald R. Griffin. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts (1984), ix, +237., Price £6.75 (paperback) , 1986 .

[45]  S. Shettleworth Cognition, evolution, and behavior , 1998 .

[46]  George Adelman,et al.  Encyclopedia of neuroscience , 2004 .

[47]  Holk Cruse,et al.  No Need for a Cognitive Map: Decentralized Memory for Insect Navigation , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[48]  P. Colgan,et al.  Animal Homing , 1992, Chapman & Hall Animal Behaviour Series.

[49]  Andrea D. Székely,et al.  The avian hippocampal formation: subdivisions and connectivity , 1999, Behavioural Brain Research.

[50]  A. Einstein On the Method of Theoretical Physics , 1934, Philosophy of Science.

[51]  D. Biro,et al.  Homing pigeons develop local route stereotypy , 2005, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[52]  Ingo Schiffner,et al.  Strategies of Young Pigeons during ‘Map’ Learning , 2011, Journal of Navigation.

[53]  G. Vallortigara,et al.  From natural geometry to spatial cognition , 2012, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[54]  Lynn Nadel,et al.  Children's Use of Landmarks: Implications for Modularity Theory , 2002, Psychological science.

[55]  Dora Biro,et al.  Familiar route loyalty implies visual pilotage in the homing pigeon. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[56]  H. Pick,et al.  The development of children's representations of large-scale environments. , 1978 .

[57]  Brad E. Pfeiffer,et al.  Hippocampal place cell sequences depict future paths to remembered goals , 2013, Nature.

[58]  Giorgio Vallortigara,et al.  Olfactory lateralization in homing pigeons: a GPS study on birds released with unilateral olfactory inputs , 2011, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[59]  R. Menzel,et al.  Honey bees navigate according to a map-like spatial memory. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[60]  E. Tolman Cognitive maps in rats and men. , 1948, Psychological review.

[61]  M. Lindauer,et al.  Sonnenorientierung der Bienen unter der Äquatorsonne und zur Nachtzeit , 2004, Naturwissenschaften.

[62]  G. Dell’Omo,et al.  Finding home: the final step of the pigeons' homing process studied with a GPS data logger , 2007, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[63]  Francisco J. Silva,et al.  Humans’ folk physics is not enough to explain variations in their tool-using behavior , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[64]  S. Shettleworth Cognition, evolution, and behavior, 2nd ed. , 2010 .

[65]  SIMON BENHAMOU,et al.  No evidence for cognitive mapping in rats , 1996, Animal Behaviour.

[66]  Thomas S. Collett,et al.  Rapid Navigational Learning in Insects with a Short Lifespan , 1998, Connect. Sci..

[67]  J. O’Keefe,et al.  Boundary Vector Cells in the Subiculum of the Hippocampal Formation , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[68]  Ingo Schiffner,et al.  Homing flights of pigeons over familiar terrain , 2007, Animal Behaviour.

[69]  Anna Waisman,et al.  Flexibility of cue use in the fox squirrel (Sciurus niger) , 2008, Animal Cognition.

[70]  Gerald E. Hough,et al.  Internal connectivity of the homing pigeon (Columba livia) hippocampal formation: An anterograde and retrograde tracer study , 2003, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[71]  A. Bennett,et al.  Do animals have cognitive maps? , 1996, The Journal of experimental biology.

[72]  R. Menzel,et al.  A Common Frame of Reference for Learned and Communicated Vectors in Honeybee Navigation , 2011, Current Biology.

[73]  A C Kamil,et al.  Way-finding and landmarks: the multiple-bearings hypothesis. , 2001, The Journal of experimental biology.

[74]  E. Visalberghi,et al.  Lack of comprehension of cause-effect relations in tool-using capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). , 1994, Journal of comparative psychology.

[75]  Randolf Menzel,et al.  Dominance of the odometer over serial landmark learning in honeybee navigation , 2010, Naturwissenschaften.

[76]  Cristina Broglio,et al.  Evolution of Forebrain and Spatial Cognition in Vertebrates: Conservation across Diversity , 2003, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.

[77]  P. Nagel,et al.  Spatio-temporal use of the urban habitat by feral pigeons (Columba livia) , 2006, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[78]  Tim Guilford,et al.  Individual strategies and release site features determine the extent of deviation in clock-shifted pigeons at familiar sites , 2013, Animal Cognition.

[79]  Daniele Nardi,et al.  Reorienting with terrain slope and landmarks , 2013, Memory & cognition.

[80]  Denise Manahan-Vaughan,et al.  The hippocampal CA1 region and dentate gyrus differentiate between environmental and spatial feature encoding through long-term depression. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[81]  Daniele Nardi,et al.  Slope-driven goal location behavior in pigeons. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[82]  Lucia F. Jacobs,et al.  Visual environment and delay affect cache retrieval accuracy in a food-storing rodent , 1998 .

[83]  D. Olton,et al.  Neurobiology of Comparative Cognition , 1990 .

[84]  Karl Deisseroth,et al.  Optetrode: a multichannel readout for optogenetic control in freely moving mice , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[85]  Anna Gagliardo,et al.  Forty years of olfactory navigation in birds , 2013, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[86]  Giorgio Vallortigara,et al.  Spatial reorientation in large and small enclosures: comparative and developmental perspectives , 2008, Cognitive Processing.

[87]  R. Holland,et al.  The role of visual landmarks in the avian familiar area map , 2003, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[88]  M. Yartsev,et al.  Grid cells without theta oscillations in the entorhinal cortex of bats , 2011, Nature.

[89]  N. Newcombe,et al.  Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? squaring theory and evidence , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[90]  Verner P. Bingman,et al.  Hippocampal ablated homing pigeons show a persistent impairment in the time taken to return home , 1988, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[91]  Nachum Ulanovsky,et al.  Neuroscience: How Is Three-Dimensional Space Encoded in the Brain? , 2011, Current Biology.

[92]  Matthew Collett,et al.  How desert ants use a visual landmark for guidance along a habitual route , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[93]  E. Maguire,et al.  The Human Hippocampus and Spatial and Episodic Memory , 2002, Neuron.

[94]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep. , 1994, Science.

[95]  Andrew Philippides,et al.  How might ants use panoramic views for route navigation? , 2011, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[96]  G. Handelmann,et al.  A re-examination of the role of hippocampus in working memory , 1979 .

[97]  R. W. Fitzgerald Sex Differences in Spatial Ability: An Evolutionary Hypothesis and Test , 1986, The American Naturalist.

[98]  Ingo Schiffner,et al.  Development of the navigational system in homing pigeons: increase in complexity of the navigational map , 2013, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[99]  Onur Güntürkün,et al.  Navigation‐induced ZENK expression in the olfactory system of pigeons (Columba livia) , 2010, The European journal of neuroscience.

[100]  Lucia F. Jacobs,et al.  See Blockindiscussions, Blockinstats, Blockinand Blockinauthor Blockinprofiles Blockinfor Blockinthis Blockinpublication the Blockinecology Blockinof Blockinspatial Blockincognition , 2022 .

[101]  Bruce L. McNaughton,et al.  Spatial representation in the rat: Conceptual, behavioral, and neurophysiological perspectives , 1990 .

[102]  A. Etienne,et al.  Navigation through vector addition , 1998, Nature.

[103]  D. Nardi,et al.  The world is not flat: can people reorient using slope? , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[104]  Martin Wikelski,et al.  Going wild: what a global small-animal tracking system could do for experimental biologists , 2007, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[105]  Jacques Bovet,et al.  Strategies of homing behavior in the red squirrel, Tamiasciurus hudsonicus , 1984, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[106]  Lucia F Jacobs,et al.  From Movement to Transitivity: The Role of Hippocampal Parallel Maps in Configural Learni , 2006, Reviews in the neurosciences.

[107]  V P Bingman,et al.  The importance of comparative studies and ecological validity for understanding hippocampal structure and cognitive function , 1992, Hippocampus.

[108]  Bita Moghaddam,et al.  A Mechanistic Approach to Preventing Schizophrenia in At-Risk Individuals , 2013, Neuron.

[109]  M. Heisenberg Mushroom body memoir: from maps to models , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[110]  Ronald L. Davis,et al.  Traces of Drosophila Memory , 2011, Neuron.

[111]  Valeria Anna Sovrano,et al.  Spatial reorientation: the effects of space size on the encoding of landmark and geometry information , 2007, Animal Cognition.

[112]  G. Striedter Principles of brain evolution. , 2005 .

[113]  Valeria Anna Sovrano,et al.  Dissecting the Geometric Module , 2006, Psychological science.

[114]  Angus J. C. McMorland,et al.  Exploratory behaviour of colonizing rats in novel environments , 2010, Animal Behaviour.

[115]  Wei Zhang,et al.  Experience Improves Feature Extraction in Drosophila , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[116]  G. Handelmann,et al.  Hippocampus, space, and memory , 1979 .

[117]  Julia Fischer,et al.  Animal Thinking: Contemporary Issues in Comparative Cognition , 2011 .

[118]  Verner P Bingman,et al.  Lateralized functional components of spatial cognition in the avian hippocampal formation: Evidence from single‐unit recordings in freely moving homing pigeons , 2006, Hippocampus.

[119]  Stanley Heinze,et al.  Central neural coding of sky polarization in insects , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[120]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  Hippocampal representation in place learning , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[121]  Paul Ellen,et al.  Problem solving in the rat: Piecemeal acquisition of cognitive maps , 1984 .

[122]  R. Passingham The hippocampus as a cognitive map J. O'Keefe & L. Nadel, Oxford University Press, Oxford (1978). 570 pp., £25.00 , 1979, Neuroscience.

[123]  S. Gaulin,et al.  Sexual selection for spatial-learning ability , 1989, Animal Behaviour.

[124]  Benjamin J. Kraus,et al.  Hippocampal “Time Cells”: Time versus Path Integration , 2013, Neuron.

[125]  Melissa S. Bowlin,et al.  Evidence for a navigational map stretching across the continental U.S. in a migratory songbird , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[126]  V. Bingman,et al.  Unimpaired acquisition of spatial reference memory, but impaired homing performance in hippocampal-ablated pigeons , 1988, Behavioural Brain Research.

[127]  Roddy M. Grieves,et al.  Cognitive maps and spatial inference in animals: Rats fail to take a novel shortcut, but can take a previously experienced one , 2013 .

[128]  Dora Biro,et al.  Homing Pigeons Respond to Time-Compensated Solar Cues Even in Sight of the Loft , 2013, PloS one.

[129]  R. Kesner,et al.  Implications of CA3 NMDA and opiate receptors for spatial pattern completion in rats , 2009, Hippocampus.

[130]  Lucia F Jacobs,et al.  Unpacking the cognitive map: the parallel map theory of hippocampal function. , 2003, Psychological review.

[131]  Mary A. Peterson,et al.  Cognitive Biology: Evolutionary and Developmental Perspectives on Mind, Brain, and Behavior , 2009 .

[132]  Emilio Kropff,et al.  Place cells, grid cells, and the brain's spatial representation system. , 2008, Annual review of neuroscience.

[133]  N. Schmajuk Cognitive maps , 1998 .