Surprise is predicted by event probability, outcome valence, outcome meaningfulness, and gender

The present research was designed to better understand how the magnitude of experienced surprise is affected by both individual difference variables as well as variations in surprise-eliciting stimuli. Eighty-five participants played 5 versions of a slot machine-like game. The five games only differed with respect to the probability of winning each trial—10 % (i.e., wins were highly unusual), 30, 50, 70, and 90 % (i.e., losses were highly unusual). Players were given a fictitious “bankroll” at the beginning of each game and played up to 25 trials of each game. On each trial, players selected their wager amount, “pulled” the handle, learned the outcome (win or loss), and reported their surprise level using a 1 (none) to 9 (extremely) Likert scale. Replicating past research, results revealed that self-reported surprise was inversely related to outcome probability and that wins were rated as more surprising than losses, even when wins and losses occurred at the same level of probability. Novel results include finding that larger wagers predicted greater felt surprise (regardless of outcome), and that women reported greater surprise to both wins and losses than men.

[1]  D. Mandel Violations of coherence in subjective probability: A representational and assessment processes account , 2008, Cognition.

[2]  Pierre Baldi,et al.  Of bits and wows: A Bayesian theory of surprise with applications to attention , 2010, Neural Networks.

[3]  B. Bilgin,et al.  Losses loom more likely than gains: Propensity to imagine losses increases their subjective probability , 2012 .

[4]  A. Kring,et al.  Sex differences in emotion: expression, experience, and physiology. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[5]  R. Plutchik A GENERAL PSYCHOEVOLUTIONARY THEORY OF EMOTION , 1980 .

[6]  H. Demaree,et al.  Trait dominance predicts risk-taking , 2009 .

[7]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  A new look at anchoring effects: basic anchoring and its antecedents. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[8]  J. Hraba,et al.  Gender, gambling and problem gambling , 1996, Journal of Gambling Studies.

[9]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect Theory : An Analysis of Decision under Risk Author ( s ) : , 2007 .

[10]  C. Darwin The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals , .

[11]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect theory: analysis of decision under risk , 1979 .

[12]  William D. Schafer,et al.  Gender differences in risk taking: A meta-analysis. , 1999 .

[13]  Pierre Baldi,et al.  Bayesian surprise attracts human attention , 2005, Vision Research.

[14]  R. Reisenzein Exploring the Strength of Association between the Components of Emotion Syndromes: The Case of Surprise , 2000 .

[15]  C. W. Hughes Emotion: Theory, Research and Experience , 1982 .

[16]  W. Meyer,et al.  Toward a Process Analysis of Emotions: The Case of Surprise , 1997 .

[17]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases , 1974, Science.

[18]  S. Mineka,et al.  Temperament, personality, and the mood and anxiety disorders. , 1994, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[19]  Udo Rudolph,et al.  Temporal characteristics of the surprise reaction induced by schema-discrepant visual and auditory events , 1994 .

[20]  Gideon Keren,et al.  When are successes more surprising than failures? , 2002 .

[21]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty , 1982 .

[22]  Brian Knutson,et al.  Neural Antecedents of Financial Decisions , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  H. Demaree,et al.  Risk dishabituation: in repeated gambling, risk is reduced following low-probability "surprising" events (wins or losses). , 2012, Emotion.

[24]  P. Ekman An argument for basic emotions , 1992 .

[25]  David V. Budescu,et al.  The elusive wishful thinking effect , 1995 .

[26]  R. Baayen,et al.  Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items , 2008 .

[27]  L. Brody,et al.  Gender, emotional expression, and parent–child boundaries. , 1996 .

[28]  M. Bradley,et al.  Emotion and motivation II: sex differences in picture processing. , 2001, Emotion.

[29]  H. Demaree,et al.  You bet: How personality differences affect risk-taking preferences , 2008 .

[30]  Camelia M. Kuhnen,et al.  The Neural Basis of Financial Risk Taking , 2005, Neuron.

[31]  E. Brandstätter,et al.  A cognitive‐emotional account of the shape of the probability weighting function , 2002 .

[32]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Loss Aversion Is an Affective Forecasting Error , 2006, Psychological science.

[33]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[34]  Ruma Falk,et al.  Judgment of Coincidences: Mine versus Yours , 1989 .

[35]  G. R. Frisch,et al.  Gambling Behavior and Pathology in Relation to Impulsivity, Sensation Seeking, and Risky Behavior in Male College Students , 1998, Journal of Gambling Studies.

[36]  Guido H. E. Gendolla,et al.  Surprise in the Context of Achievement: The Role of Outcome Valence and Importance , 1997 .

[37]  Guido H. E. Gendolla,et al.  Surprise and Motivation of Causal Search: How Are They Affected by Outcome Valence and Importance? , 2001 .

[38]  K. Teigen,et al.  Surprises: low probabilities or high contrasts? , 2003, Cognition.

[39]  J. Derevensky,et al.  Personality characteristics and risk-taking tendencies among adolescent gamblers. , 2006 .

[40]  P. Lang,et al.  International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Instruction Manual and Affective Ratings (Tech. Rep. No. A-4) , 1999 .

[41]  Udo Rudolph,et al.  An experimental analysis of surprise , 1991 .

[42]  R. D. Kavanaugh,et al.  Emotion : interdisciplinary perspectives , 1996 .

[43]  K. Scherer,et al.  Beyond surprise: the puzzle of infants' expressive reactions to expectancy violation. , 2004, Emotion.

[44]  David Ansic,et al.  Gender differences in risk behaviour in financial decision-making: An experimental analysis. , 1997 .