Perceptual and cognitive factors governing performance in comparative arrival-time judgments.

Four experiments were conducted to investigate factors affecting relative arrival-time judgments in the transverse plane. Across experiments, results indicated an overreliance on relative distance information. The levels of relative velocity and distance used in the arrival-time task were proved discriminable, and performance in both relative velocity and distance judgments predicted performance in the relative arrival-time task. Despite the distance bias, an attempt to integrate relative velocity and distance information was also evidenced. The distance bias appears to have resulted from resource limitations on the concurrent processing of relative velocity and distance information, causing relative velocity information to become resource limited. The final experiment assessed the stability of performance in each of the tasks over time and provided evidence of individual differences in the ability to coordinate information from multiple sources.

[1]  J P Wann,et al.  Time-to-contact judgment in the locomotion of adults and preschool children. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  J. Pellegrino,et al.  Complex Information Coordination Performance: Differential Changes in Working Memory Contributions Following Training. Cognitive Components of Information Coordination , 1993 .

[3]  James W. Pellegrino,et al.  Comparing the Tortoise and the Hare: Gender Differences and Experience in Dynamic Spatial Reasoning Tasks , 1993 .

[4]  M K Kaiser,et al.  Optical specification of time-to-passage: observers' sensitivity to global tau. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  M. Just,et al.  From the SelectedWorks of Marcel Adam Just 1992 A capacity theory of comprehension : Individual differences in working memory , 2017 .

[6]  Earl Hunt,et al.  Coordinating cognitive information: Task effects and individual differences in integrating information from several sources , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.

[7]  D. Gilden On the origins of dynamical awareness. , 1991, Psychological review.

[8]  J. Tresilian,et al.  Empirical and theoretical issues in the perception of time to contact. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  P R DeLucia,et al.  Pictorial and motion-based information for depth perception. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  Kevin A. Morrin,et al.  Individual Differences in Real-Time Information Coordination: Relating Dynamic Spatial and Verbal Information , 1990 .

[11]  Gary E. Riccio,et al.  Responses to Optical Looming in the Retinal Center and Periphery , 1990 .

[12]  W Schiff,et al.  Accuracy of judging time to arrival: effects of modality, trajectory, and gender. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  R. Bootsma,et al.  Timing an attacking forehand drive in table tennis. , 1990 .

[14]  M. Posner Foundations of cognitive science , 1989 .

[15]  Allen Allport,et al.  Visual attention , 1989 .

[16]  D R Proffitt,et al.  Understanding collision dynamics. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  H. Heuer,et al.  Perspectives on Perception and Action , 1989 .

[18]  V Cavallo,et al.  Visual Information and Skill Level in Time-To-Collision Estimation , 1988, Perception.

[19]  W. Simpson,et al.  Depth Discrimination from Optic Flow , 1988, Perception.

[20]  James W. Pellegrino,et al.  The ability to reason about movement in the visual field , 1988 .

[21]  D. Navon,et al.  Role of outcome conflict in dual-task interference. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[22]  W H Warren,et al.  The Way the Ball Bounces: Visual and Auditory Perception of Elasticity and Control of the Bounce Pass , 1987, Perception.

[23]  C. Clifton,et al.  The independence of syntactic processing , 1986 .

[24]  James E. Cutting,et al.  Perception with an eye for motion , 1986 .

[25]  David N. Lee,et al.  Visual Timing of Interceptive Action , 1985 .

[26]  D. Ingle,et al.  Brain mechanisms and spatial vision , 1985 .

[27]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Deciding the Existence of a Time-Sharing Ability: A Combined Methodological and Theoretical Approach , 1984 .

[28]  H. Ross,et al.  Optic-Flow and Cognitive Factors in Time-to-Collision Estimates , 1983, Perception.

[29]  J. Fodor The Modularity of mind. An essay on faculty psychology , 1986 .

[30]  J T Todd,et al.  Visual Perception of Relative Mass in Dynamic Events , 1982, Perception.

[31]  J T Todd,et al.  Visual information about moving objects. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[32]  G. Stelmach,et al.  Tutorials in Motor Behavior , 1980 .

[33]  W Schiff,et al.  Information Used in Judging Impending Collision , 1979, Perception.

[34]  Daniel Gopher,et al.  On the Economy of the Human Processing System: A Model of Multiple Capacity. , 1977 .

[35]  David N. Lee,et al.  A Theory of Visual Control of Braking Based on Information about Time-to-Collision , 1976, Perception.

[36]  Daniel G Bobrow,et al.  On data-limited and resource-limited processes , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[37]  W. Schiff PERCEPTION OF IMPENDING COLLISION: A STUDY OF VISUALLY DIRECTED AVOIDANT BEHAVIOR. , 1965, Psychological monographs.

[38]  George Sperling,et al.  The information available in brief visual presentations. , 1960 .

[39]  R. V. Edwards,et al.  The prediction of collision. , 1957, The American journal of psychology.

[40]  B. Fruchter,et al.  Some perceptual factors measured by motion picture tests. , 1952 .