Neural Correlates of Contrast Detection at Threshold

Human psychophysical studies have demonstrated that, for stimuli near the threshold of visibility, detection of motion in one direction is unaffected by the superimposition of motion in the opposite direction. To investigate the neural basis for this perceptual phenomenon, we recorded from directionally selective neurons in macaque visual area MT (middle temporal visual area). Contrast thresholds obtained for single gratings moving in a neuron's preferred direction were compared with those obtained for motion presented simultaneously in the neuron's preferred and antipreferred directions. A simple model based on probability summation between neurons tuned to opposite directions could sufficiently account for contrast thresholds revealed psychophysically, suggesting that area MT is likely to provide the neural basis for contrast detection of stimuli modulated in time.

[1]  D. Heeger Modeling simple-cell direction selectivity with normalized, half-squared, linear operators. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  C. Stromeyer,et al.  Motion detection on flashed, stationary pedestal gratings: Evidence for an opponent-motion mechanism , 1998, Vision Research.

[3]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Responses of MT and MST neurons to one and two moving objects in the receptive field. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  D. G. Albrecht,et al.  Cortical neurons: Isolation of contrast gain control , 1992, Vision Research.

[5]  Denis G. Pelli,et al.  Accurate control of contrast on microcomputer displays , 1991, Vision Research.

[6]  J. Movshon,et al.  The statistical reliability of signals in single neurons in cat and monkey visual cortex , 1983, Vision Research.

[7]  O. Braddick,et al.  Responses to Opposed Directions of Motion: Continuum or Independent Mechanisms? , 1996, Vision Research.

[8]  D. Kiper,et al.  Chromatic properties of neurons in macaque area V2 , 1997, Visual Neuroscience.

[9]  R A Andersen,et al.  Transparent motion perception as detection of unbalanced motion signals. I. Psychophysics , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[10]  T D Albright,et al.  What happens if it changes color when it moves?: the nature of chromatic input to macaque visual area MT , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[11]  R. Shapley,et al.  Linear mechanisms of directional selectivity in simple cells of cat striate cortex. , 1987, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[12]  R. L. de Valois,et al.  Psychophysical studies of monkey vision. 3. Spatial luminance contrast sensitivity tests of macaque and human observers. , 1974, Vision research.

[13]  Jacob Nachmias,et al.  On the psychometric function for contrast detection , 1981, Vision Research.

[14]  Karen R. Dobkins,et al.  Infant contrast detectors are selective for direction of motion , 1996, Vision Research.

[15]  Brian J. Murphy,et al.  Summation and discrimination of gratings moving in opposite directions , 1980, Vision Research.

[16]  J. Movshon,et al.  Chromatic properties of neurons in macaque MT , 1994, Visual Neuroscience.

[17]  The role of cortical directional selectivity in detection of motion and flicker , 1986, Vision Research.

[18]  Andrew T. Smith,et al.  Visual detection of motion , 1994 .

[19]  D. Heeger,et al.  Motion Opponency in Visual Cortex , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  D. M. Green,et al.  Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .

[21]  J. van Santen,et al.  Elaborated Reichardt detectors. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[22]  A. Watson Probability summation over time , 1979, Vision Research.

[23]  E. Adelson,et al.  The analysis of moving visual patterns , 1985 .

[24]  D. G. Albrecht,et al.  Motion selectivity and the contrast-response function of simple cells in the visual cortex , 1991, Visual Neuroscience.

[25]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Visual thresholds for shearing motion in monkey and man , 1985, Vision Research.

[26]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  A model of neuronal responses in visual area MT , 1998, Vision Research.

[27]  R. Andersen,et al.  Transparent motion perception as detection of unbalanced motion signals. II. Physiology , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[28]  D. Tolhurst,et al.  Evaluation of a linear model of directional selectivity in simple cells of the cat's striate cortex , 1991, Visual Neuroscience.

[29]  D. Heeger,et al.  Contrast normalization and a linear model for the directional selectivity of simple cells in cat striate cortex , 1997, Visual Neuroscience.

[30]  Chuan Yi Tang,et al.  A 2.|E|-Bit Distributed Algorithm for the Directed Euler Trail Problem , 1993, Inf. Process. Lett..

[31]  R. Sekuler,et al.  The independence of channels in human vision selective for direction of movement. , 1975, The Journal of physiology.

[32]  W. Weibull A Statistical Distribution Function of Wide Applicability , 1951 .

[33]  E H Adelson,et al.  Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[34]  S. Klein,et al.  Opponent-movement mechanisms in human vision. , 1984, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[35]  N. Graham Visual Pattern Analyzers , 1989 .

[36]  Quick Rf A vector-magnitude model of contrast detection. , 1974 .

[37]  R A Andersen,et al.  The response of area MT and V1 neurons to transparent motion , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[38]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Coding of image contrast in central visual pathways of the macaque monkey , 1990, Vision Research.