Optimal, resource-rational or sub-optimal? Insights from cognitive development
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] M. Nardini,et al. Fusion of visual cues is not mandatory in children , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[2] D. Amso,et al. Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching , 2006, Neuropsychologia.
[3] M. Nardini,et al. When vision is not an option: children’s integration of auditory and haptic information is suboptimal , 2014, Developmental science.
[4] Pete R. Jones,et al. Development of Cue Integration in Human Navigation , 2008, Current Biology.
[5] P. Jusczyk,et al. Infants′ Detection of the Sound Patterns of Words in Fluent Speech , 1995, Cognitive Psychology.
[6] A. Kersten,et al. Less Really Is More for Adults Learning a Miniature Artificial Language , 2001 .
[7] Konrad Paul Kording,et al. Learning Priors for Bayesian Computations in the Nervous System , 2010, PloS one.
[8] J. Elman. Learning and development in neural networks: the importance of starting small , 1993, Cognition.
[9] S. Crewther,et al. The race that precedes coactivation: development of multisensory facilitation in children. , 2009, Developmental science.
[10] Michael S Landy,et al. Combining Priors and Noisy Visual Cues in a Rapid Pointing Task , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[11] C. Nelson,et al. The functional emergence of prefrontally-guided working memory systems in four- to eight-year-old children , 1998, Neuropsychologia.
[12] Konrad Paul Kording,et al. Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning , 2004, Nature.
[13] R N Aslin,et al. Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996, Science.
[14] A. Meltzoff,et al. The bimodal perception of speech in infancy. , 1982, Science.
[15] Michael N. Shadlen,et al. Temporal context calibrates interval timing , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.
[16] Sandra Weintraub,et al. II. NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): measuring executive function and attention. , 2013, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.
[17] D. Knill,et al. The brain uses adaptive internal models of scene statistics for sensorimotor estimation and planning , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[18] Carla L. Hudson Kam,et al. Regularizing Unpredictable Variation: The Roles of Adult and Child Learners in Language Formation and Change , 2005 .
[19] Konrad Paul Kording,et al. The development of Bayesian integration in sensorimotor estimation , 2017, bioRxiv.
[20] Fei Xu,et al. Intuitive statistics by 8-month-old infants , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[21] Scott P. Johnson,et al. Visual statistical learning in infancy: evidence for a domain general learning mechanism , 2002, Cognition.
[22] Eero P. Simoncelli,et al. Noise characteristics and prior expectations in human visual speed perception , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.
[23] Richard N. Aslin,et al. Learning and inference using complex generative models in a spatial localization task , 2016, Journal of vision.
[24] Elissa L. Newport,et al. Maturational Constraints on Language Learning , 1990, Cogn. Sci..
[25] David C. Burr,et al. Young Children Do Not Integrate Visual and Haptic Form Information , 2008, Current Biology.
[26] Shinsuke Shimojo,et al. Development of multisensory spatial integration and perception in humans. , 2006, Developmental science.
[27] P. H. Miller,et al. A developmental perspective on executive function. , 2010, Child development.