Toward a Rational and Mechanistic Account of Mental Effort.

In spite of its familiar phenomenology, the mechanistic basis for mental effort remains poorly understood. Although most researchers agree that mental effort is aversive and stems from limitations in our capacity to exercise cognitive control, it is unclear what gives rise to those limitations and why they result in an experience of control as costly. The presence of these control costs also raises further questions regarding how best to allocate mental effort to minimize those costs and maximize the attendant benefits. This review explores recent advances in computational modeling and empirical research aimed at addressing these questions at the level of psychological process and neural mechanism, examining both the limitations to mental effort exertion and how we manage those limited cognitive resources. We conclude by identifying remaining challenges for theoretical accounts of mental effort as well as possible applications of the available findings to understanding the causes of and potential solutions for apparent failures to exert the mental effort required of us.

[1]  E. Rowland Theory of Games and Economic Behavior , 1946, Nature.

[2]  H. Simon,et al.  Rational choice and the structure of the environment. , 1956, Psychological review.

[3]  G. A. Miller THE PSYCHOLOGICAL REVIEW THE MAGICAL NUMBER SEVEN, PLUS OR MINUS TWO: SOME LIMITS ON OUR CAPACITY FOR PROCESSING INFORMATION 1 , 1956 .

[4]  John McCarthy,et al.  Programs with common sense , 1960 .

[5]  D Kahneman,et al.  Pupil Diameter and Load on Memory , 1966, Science.

[6]  S. Sternberg Memory-scanning: mental processes revealed by reaction-time experiments. , 1969, American scientist.

[7]  D. Allport,et al.  On the Division of Attention: A Disproof of the Single Channel Hypothesis , 1972, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[8]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Attention and Effort , 1973 .

[9]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases , 1974, Science.

[10]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and cognitive control. , 1975 .

[11]  Daniel Gopher,et al.  On the Economy of the Human Processing System: A Model of Multiple Capacity. , 1977 .

[12]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. , 1977 .

[13]  L. Beach,et al.  A Contingency Model for the Selection of Decision Strategies , 1978 .

[14]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .

[15]  John McCarthy,et al.  SOME PHILOSOPHICAL PROBLEMS FROM THE STANDPOINT OF ARTI CIAL INTELLIGENCE , 1987 .

[16]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  The need for cognition. , 1982 .

[17]  G. Logan Skill and automaticity: Relations, implications, and future directions. , 1985 .

[18]  James L. McClelland,et al.  PDP models and general issues in cognitive science , 1986 .

[19]  Eric J. Johnson,et al.  Adaptive Strategy Selection in Decision Making. , 1988 .

[20]  A. Jensen,et al.  Human abilities in cultural context: Speed of information processing and population differences , 1988 .

[21]  W. Mischel,et al.  Delay of gratification in children. , 1989, Science.

[22]  J. S. Nairne A feature model of immediate memory , 1990, Memory & cognition.

[23]  James L. McClelland,et al.  On the control of automatic processes: a parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. , 1990, Psychological review.

[24]  Eric Horvitz,et al.  Time-Dependent Utility and Action Under Uncertainty , 1991, UAI.

[25]  Evidence for a close relationship between Iostephane and Viguiera (Asteraceae: Heliantheae). , 1991 .

[26]  Stuart J. Russell,et al.  Principles of Metareasoning , 1989, Artif. Intell..

[27]  R. Eisenberger Learned industriousness. , 1992, Psychological review.

[28]  Devika Subramanian,et al.  Provably Bounded Optimal Agents , 1993, IJCAI.

[29]  J. Elman Learning and development in neural networks: the importance of starting small , 1993, Cognition.

[30]  D. Alan Allport,et al.  SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .

[31]  G. A. Miller The magical number seven plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. , 1956, Psychological review.

[32]  John R. Anderson ACT: A simple theory of complex cognition. , 1996 .

[33]  R. Baumeister,et al.  Self-Regulation Failure: An Overview , 1996 .

[34]  G. R. J. Hockey Compensatory control in the regulation of human performance under stress and high workload: A cognitive-energetical framework , 1997, Biological Psychology.

[35]  Edward K. Vogel,et al.  The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions , 1997, Nature.

[36]  H. Gr Compensatory control in the regulation of human performance under stress and high workload; a cognitive-energetical framework. , 1997 .

[37]  D. Norris,et al.  The primacy model: a new model of immediate serial recall. , 1998, Psychological review.

[38]  Rich Caruana,et al.  Multitask Learning , 1998, Encyclopedia of Machine Learning and Data Mining.

[39]  R. Baumeister,et al.  Ego depletion: is the active self a limited resource? , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[40]  R. Baumeister,et al.  Self-control as limited resource: regulatory depletion patterns. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[41]  Colin Camerer,et al.  The Effects of Financial Incentives in Experiments: A Review and Capital-Labor-Production Framework , 1999 .

[42]  G. Wylie,et al.  Task-switching: Positive and negative priming of task-set. , 1999 .

[43]  E. Montgomery,et al.  The Subthalamic Nucleus , 2014 .

[44]  M. Posner,et al.  Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[45]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[46]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[47]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Attention and performance. , 2001, Annual review of psychology.

[48]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. , 2002, Psychological review.

[49]  Geoffrey B. Sprinkle,et al.  The effects of monetary incentives on effort and task performance: theories, evidence, and a framework for research , 2002 .

[50]  D. Kahneman A perspective on judgment and choice: mapping bounded rationality. , 2003, The American psychologist.

[51]  T. Shallice,et al.  Human cingulate cortex and autonomic control: converging neuroimaging and clinical evidence. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[52]  M. Walton,et al.  Action sets and decisions in the medial frontal cortex , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[53]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.

[54]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.

[55]  Isbn The Principles of Psychology: Volume 1 , 2004 .

[56]  R. Baumeister,et al.  High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. , 2004, Journal of personality.

[57]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. , 2005, Annual review of neuroscience.

[58]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  Learned Predictions of Error Likelihood in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2005, Science.

[59]  Gregor Leicht,et al.  Evidence for a close relationship between conscious effort and anterior cingulate cortex activity. , 2005, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[60]  Richard S. Sutton,et al.  Reinforcement Learning: An Introduction , 1998, IEEE Trans. Neural Networks.

[61]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Journal of Economic Perspectives—Volume 19, Number 4—Fall 2005—Pages 3–24 The Vulcanization of the Human Brain: A Neural Perspective on Interactions Between Cognition and Emotion , 2022 .

[62]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Effortless control: executive attention and conscious feeling of mental effort are dissociable , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[63]  Nelson Cowan,et al.  Working Memory Capacity , 2005 .

[64]  M. Farah,et al.  Is anterior cingulate cortex necessary for cognitive control? , 2005, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[65]  Vivian Zayas,et al.  Predicting Cognitive Control From Preschool to Late Adolescence and Young Adulthood , 2006, Psychological science.

[66]  John J. Foxe,et al.  The Anterior Cingulate and Error Avoidance , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[67]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Making Working Memory Work: A Computational Model of Learning in the Prefrontal Cortex and Basal Ganglia , 2006, Neural Computation.

[68]  Klaus Oberauer,et al.  A formal model of capacity limits in working memory , 2006 .

[69]  Kristina M. Visscher,et al.  A Core System for the Implementation of Task Sets , 2006, Neuron.

[70]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The physics of optimal decision making: a formal analysis of models of performance in two-alternative forced-choice tasks. , 2006, Psychological review.

[71]  P. Dayan,et al.  Tonic dopamine: opportunity costs and the control of response vigor , 2007, Psychopharmacology.

[72]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk — Source link , 2007 .

[73]  R. Baumeister,et al.  Self-control relies on glucose as a limited energy source: willpower is more than a metaphor. , 2007, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[74]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Error-likelihood prediction in the medial frontal cortex: a critical evaluation. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[75]  M. Walton,et al.  Calculating the Cost of Acting in Frontal Cortex , 2007, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[76]  Angela L. Duckworth,et al.  Grit: perseverance and passion for long-term goals. , 2007, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[77]  A. Graybiel Habits, rituals, and the evaluative brain. , 2008, Annual review of neuroscience.

[78]  S. Petersen,et al.  A dual-networks architecture of top-down control , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[79]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Learning to Use Working Memory in Partially Observable Environments through Dopaminergic Reinforcement , 2008, NIPS.

[80]  G. Gigerenzer Rationality for Mortals: How People Cope with Uncertainty , 2008 .

[81]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Hierarchically organized behavior and its neural foundations: A reinforcement learning perspective , 2009, Cognition.

[82]  N. Chater,et al.  Précis of Bayesian Rationality: The Probabilistic Approach to Human Reasoning , 2009, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[83]  Joseph T. McGuire,et al.  Effort discounting in human nucleus accumbens , 2009, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[84]  Timothy Edward John Behrens,et al.  Effort-Based Cost–Benefit Valuation and the Human Brain , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[85]  Samuele M. Marcora,et al.  Mental fatigue impairs physical performance in humans. , 2009, Journal of applied physiology.

[86]  L. Pessoa,et al.  Combined Effects of Attention and Motivation on Visual Task Performance: Transient and Sustained Motivational Effects , 2008, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[87]  Samuele M. Marcora Perception of effort during exercise is independent of afferent feedback from skeletal muscles, heart, and lungs. , 2009, Journal of applied physiology.

[88]  J. Salamone,et al.  Dopamine, Behavioral Economics, and Effort , 2009, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[89]  E. Koechlin,et al.  Motivation and cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[90]  A. Craig,et al.  How do you feel — now? The anterior insula and human awareness , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[91]  J. Duncan The multiple-demand (MD) system of the primate brain: mental programs for intelligent behaviour , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[92]  B. Franke,et al.  Striatal Dopamine Mediates the Interface between Motivational and Cognitive Control in Humans: Evidence from Genetic Imaging , 2010, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[93]  M. Walton,et al.  Dissociable cost and benefit encoding of future rewards by mesolimbic dopamine , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[94]  Mathias Pessiglione,et al.  Separate Valuation Subsystems for Delay and Effort Decision Costs , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[95]  M. Hagger,et al.  Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: a meta-analysis. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.

[96]  Joseph T. McGuire,et al.  Prefrontal cortex, cognitive control, and the registration of decision costs , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[97]  C. N. Boehler,et al.  The influence of reward associations on conflict processing in the Stroop task , 2010, Cognition.

[98]  Joseph T. McGuire,et al.  Decision making and the avoidance of cognitive demand. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[99]  V. Menon,et al.  Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function , 2010, Brain Structure and Function.

[100]  S. Nicola The Flexible Approach Hypothesis: Unification of Effort and Cue-Responding Hypotheses for the Role of Nucleus Accumbens Dopamine in the Activation of Reward-Seeking Behavior , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[101]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Conscious perception of errors and its relation to the anterior insula , 2010, Brain Structure and Function.

[102]  Samuele M. Marcora,et al.  The limit to exercise tolerance in humans: mind over muscle? , 2010, European Journal of Applied Physiology.

[103]  H. Critchley,et al.  Conjoint activity of anterior insular and anterior cingulate cortex: awareness and response , 2010, Brain Structure and Function.

[104]  Stefan Everling,et al.  Theta-activity in anterior cingulate cortex predicts task rules and their adjustments following errors , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[105]  David K Bilkey,et al.  Neurons in the Rat Anterior Cingulate Cortex Dynamically Encode Cost–Benefit in a Spatial Decision-Making Task , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[106]  G. Robert J. Hockey,et al.  A motivational control theory of cognitive fatigue. , 2011 .

[107]  Rolf Loeber,et al.  Role of test motivation in intelligence testing , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[108]  R. Davidson,et al.  The integration of negative affect, pain and cognitive control in the cingulate cortex , 2011, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[109]  Thomas V. Wiecki,et al.  Subthalamic nucleus stimulation reverses mediofrontal influence over decision threshold , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[110]  John F. Magnotti,et al.  Visual Short-Term Memory Compared in Rhesus Monkeys and Humans , 2011, Current Biology.

[111]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Review Frontal Cortex and Reward-guided Learning and Decision-making Figure 1. Frontal Brain Regions in the Macaque Involved in Reward-guided Learning and Decision-making Finer Grained Anatomical Divisions with Frontal Cortical Systems for Reward-guided Behavior , 2022 .

[112]  Nicholas T. Franklin,et al.  Behavioral and neural correlates of delay of gratification 40 years later , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[113]  Sander Nieuwenhuis,et al.  Pupil Diameter Predicts Changes in the Exploration–Exploitation Trade-off: Evidence for the Adaptive Gain Theory , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[114]  Daniel A. Braun,et al.  Information, Utility and Bounded Rationality , 2011, AGI.

[115]  Stefan Everling,et al.  Specific Contributions of Ventromedial, Anterior Cingulate, and Lateral Prefrontal Cortex for Attentional Selection and Stimulus Valuation , 2011, PLoS biology.

[116]  T. Heatherton,et al.  Cognitive neuroscience of self-regulation failure , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[117]  Luiz Pessoa,et al.  Reward Reduces Conflict by Enhancing Attentional Control and Biasing Visual Cortical Processing , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[118]  Donald T Stuss,et al.  Functions of the Frontal Lobes: Relation to Executive Functions , 2011, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[119]  Markus Ullsperger,et al.  Surprise and Error: Common Neuronal Architecture for the Processing of Errors and Novelty , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[120]  J. Daunizeau,et al.  Neural Mechanisms Underlying Motivation of Mental Versus Physical Effort , 2012, PLoS biology.

[121]  Shaun R. Patel,et al.  Human Dorsal Anterior Cingulate Cortex Neurons Mediate Ongoing Behavioral Adaptation , 2012, Nature.

[122]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  The Phenomenology of Error Processing: The Dorsal ACC Response to Stop-signal Errors Tracks Reports of Negative Affect , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[123]  Brian P. Meier,et al.  Motivational Versus Metabolic Effects of Carbohydrates on Self-Control , 2012, Psychological science.

[124]  M. Fenske,et al.  The Unengaged Mind , 2012, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[125]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Motivation of extended behaviors by anterior cingulate cortex , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[126]  Matthew L. Dixon,et al.  The Decision to Engage Cognitive Control Is Driven by Expected Reward-Value: Neural and Behavioral Evidence , 2012, PloS one.

[127]  M. Frank,et al.  Mechanisms of hierarchical reinforcement learning in corticostriatal circuits 1: computational analysis. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[128]  Michael S Landy,et al.  Motor control is decision-making , 2012, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[129]  T. Braver The variable nature of cognitive control: a dual mechanisms framework , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[130]  P. Todd,et al.  Ecological Rationality: Intelligence in the World , 2012 .

[131]  Jeffrey N Rouder,et al.  Models of verbal working memory capacity: what does it take to make them work? , 2012, Psychological review.

[132]  Krishna P. Miyapuram Behavioral and neural correlates of delay of gratification 40 years later , 2012, Annals of Neurosciences.

[133]  M. Inzlicht,et al.  What Is Ego Depletion? Toward a Mechanistic Revision of the Resource Model of Self-Control , 2012, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[134]  Thomas L. Griffiths,et al.  "Burn-in, bias, and the rationality of anchoring" , 2012, NIPS.

[135]  David Tolpin,et al.  Selecting Computations: Theory and Applications , 2012, UAI.

[136]  Helen J. Huang,et al.  Reduction of Metabolic Cost during Motor Learning of Arm Reaching Dynamics , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[137]  Angela L. Duckworth,et al.  An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task performance. , 2013, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[138]  Pascal Vincent,et al.  Representation Learning: A Review and New Perspectives , 2012, IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence.

[139]  Daniel A. Braun,et al.  Thermodynamics as a theory of decision-making with information-processing costs , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society A: Mathematical, Physical and Engineering Sciences.

[140]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The effects of neural gain on attention and learning , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.

[141]  M. Greicius,et al.  The Will to Persevere Induced by Electrical Stimulation of the Human Cingulate Gyrus , 2013, Neuron.

[142]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The Expected Value of Control: An Integrative Theory of Anterior Cingulate Cortex Function , 2013, Neuron.

[143]  R. Turner,et al.  Limited Encoding of Effort by Dopamine Neurons in a Cost–Benefit Trade-off Task , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[144]  T. Braver,et al.  What Is the Subjective Cost of Cognitive Effort? Load, Trait, and Aging Effects Revealed by Economic Preference , 2013, PloS one.

[145]  An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task perfonnance , 2013 .

[146]  Jonathan D. Power,et al.  Control-related systems in the human brain , 2013, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[147]  Thomas L. Griffiths,et al.  Algorithm selection by rational metareasoning as a model of human strategy selection , 2014, NIPS.

[148]  Dan Zakay,et al.  Psychological time as information: the case of boredom , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[149]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The Computational and Neural Basis of Cognitive Control: Charted Territory and New Frontiers , 2014, Cogn. Sci..

[150]  M. Ullsperger,et al.  Neurophysiology of performance monitoring and adaptive behavior. , 2014, Physiological reviews.

[151]  Thomas L. Griffiths,et al.  One and Done? Optimal Decisions From Very Few Samples , 2014, Cogn. Sci..

[152]  Kobe Desender,et al.  Feeling the Conflict , 2014, Psychological science.

[153]  G. Woodman,et al.  Causal Control of Medial–Frontal Cortex Governs Electrophysiological and Behavioral Indices of Performance Monitoring and Learning , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[154]  Jean-Yves Fourniols,et al.  Wearable multi-sensor system for embedded body position and motion analysis during cycling View publication stats View publication stats , 2014 .

[155]  W. Ma,et al.  Changing concepts of working memory , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.

[156]  Simon Hanslmayr,et al.  Neural Communication Patterns Underlying Conflict Detection, Resolution, and Adaptation , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[157]  Satinder Singh,et al.  Computational Rationality: Linking Mechanism and Behavior Through Bounded Utility Maximization , 2014, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[158]  M. Frank,et al.  Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[159]  J D Cohen,et al.  Multitasking versus multiplexing: Toward a normative account of limitations in the simultaneous execution of control-demanding behaviors , 2014, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[160]  M. Frank,et al.  Conflict acts as an implicit cost in reinforcement learning , 2014, Nature Communications.

[161]  Masaaki Tanaka,et al.  Neural effects of mental fatigue caused by continuous attention load: A magnetoencephalography study , 2014, Brain Research.

[162]  M. Csíkszentmihályi,et al.  The Concept of Flow , 2014 .

[163]  M. Inzlicht,et al.  Emotional foundations of cognitive control , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[164]  William H. Alexander,et al.  Hierarchical Error Representation: A Computational Model of Anterior Cingulate and Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex , 2015, Neural Computation.

[165]  Massimo Silvetti,et al.  Adaptive effort investment in cognitive and physical tasks: a neurocomputational model , 2015, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[166]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Cognitive effort avoidance and detection in people with schizophrenia , 2014, Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience.

[167]  P. Dux,et al.  Training conquers multitasking costs by dividing task representations in the frontoparietal-subcortical system , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[168]  Thomas L. Griffiths,et al.  Rational Use of Cognitive Resources: Levels of Analysis Between the Computational and the Algorithmic , 2015, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[169]  Stefan Everling,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Cortex Cells Identify Process-Specific Errors of Attentional Control Prior to Transient Prefrontal-Cingulate Inhibition. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[170]  Robert Turner,et al.  The subthalamic nucleus during decision‐making with multiple alternatives , 2015, Human brain mapping.

[171]  M. Botvinick,et al.  A Computational Model of Control Allocation based on the Expected Value of Control , 2015 .

[172]  Evan C. Carter,et al.  A series of meta-analytic tests of the depletion effect: Self-control does not seem to rely on a limited resource. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[173]  Thomas L. Griffiths,et al.  When to use which heuristic: A rational solution to the strategy selection problem , 2015, CogSci.

[174]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Deep Learning , 2015, Nature.

[175]  Chris Arney Nudge: Improving Decisions about Health, Wealth, and Happiness , 2015 .

[176]  Samuel J. Gershman,et al.  Computational rationality: A converging paradigm for intelligence in brains, minds, and machines , 2015, Science.

[177]  David G. Rand,et al.  The evolution and devolution of cognitive control: The costs of deliberation in a competitive world , 2015, Scientific Reports.

[178]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Motivation and cognitive control: from behavior to neural mechanism. , 2015, Annual review of psychology.

[179]  Paul Schrater,et al.  Cognitive cost as dynamic allocation of energetic resources , 2015, Front. Neurosci..

[180]  E. Olivier,et al.  Disrupting the Supplementary Motor Area Makes Physical Effort Appear Less Effortful , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[181]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Task-specific effects of reward on task switching , 2014, Psychological Research.

[182]  G. Dreisbach,et al.  Conflicts as Aversive Signals for Control Adaptation , 2015 .

[183]  S. Bouret,et al.  Noradrenaline and Dopamine Neurons in the Reward/Effort Trade-Off: A Direct Electrophysiological Comparison in Behaving Monkeys , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[184]  M. Inzlicht,et al.  What does cognitive control feel like? Effective and ineffective cognitive control is associated with divergent phenomenology. , 2015, Psychophysiology.

[185]  S. Floresco,et al.  Dopamine Antagonism Decreases Willingness to Expend Physical, But Not Cognitive, Effort: A Comparison of Two Rodent Cost/Benefit Decision-Making Tasks , 2015, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[186]  T. Braver,et al.  Cognitive effort: A neuroeconomic approach , 2015, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[187]  Samuel M. McClure,et al.  Hierarchical control over effortful behavior by rodent medial frontal cortex: A computational model. , 2015, Psychological review.

[188]  Roshan Cools,et al.  The costs and benefits of brain dopamine for cognitive control. , 2016, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[189]  Evan F. Risko,et al.  Metacognitive evaluation in the avoidance of demand. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[190]  Benjamin Y Hayden,et al.  Dorsal Anterior Cingulate Cortex: A Bottom-Up View. , 2016, Annual review of neuroscience.

[191]  Timothy Edward John Behrens,et al.  Individual Differences in Premotor Brain Systems Underlie Behavioral Apathy , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[192]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Boredom, Information-Seeking and Exploration , 2016, CogSci.

[193]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Information-Seeking, Learning and the Marginal Value Theorem: A Normative Approach to Adaptive Exploration , 2016, CogSci.

[194]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Controlled vs. Automatic Processing: A Graph-Theoretic Approach to the Analysis of Serial vs. Parallel Processing in Neural Network Architectures , 2016, CogSci.

[195]  Falk Lieder,et al.  Helping people make better decisions using optimal gamification , 2016, CogSci.

[196]  M. Inzlicht,et al.  Error-related electromyographic activity over the corrugator supercilii is associated with neural performance monitoring. , 2016, Psychophysiology.

[197]  C. Li,et al.  Causal Interactions Within a Frontal-Cingulate-Parietal Network During Cognitive Control: Convergent Evidence from a Multisite-Multitask Investigation. , 2016, Cerebral cortex.

[198]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and the value of control , 2016, Nature Neuroscience.

[199]  Benjamin Naumann The Architecture Of Cognition , 2016 .

[200]  Brent A. Vogt,et al.  Midcingulate cortex: Structure, connections, homologies, functions and diseases , 2016, Journal of Chemical Neuroanatomy.

[201]  C. Holroyd The waste disposal problem of effortful control , 2016 .

[202]  Simon Farrell,et al.  The Playing Field : Findings for Evaluating Hypotheses About WM Capacity , 2016 .

[203]  Miguel A. Vadillo,et al.  The Bitter Truth About Sugar and Willpower , 2016, Psychological science.

[204]  M. Inzlicht,et al.  A Multilab Preregistered Replication of the Ego-Depletion Effect , 2016, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[205]  Mathias Pessiglione,et al.  Neural mechanisms underlying the impact of daylong cognitive work on economic decisions , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[206]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The research domain criteria framework: The case for anterior cingulate cortex , 2016, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[207]  M. Ding,et al.  Compensatory Neural Activity in Response to Cognitive Fatigue , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[208]  J. Salamone,et al.  Activational and effort-related aspects of motivation: neural mechanisms and implications for psychopathology. , 2016, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[209]  Ernst Fehr,et al.  Irrational time allocation in decision-making , 2016, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[210]  N. Crone,et al.  Cascade of neural processing orchestrates cognitive control in human frontal cortex , 2016, eLife.

[211]  T. Braver,et al.  Dopamine Does Double Duty in Motivating Cognitive Effort , 2016, Neuron.

[212]  Parallel Processing Capability Versus Efficiency of Representation in Neural Networks , 2016 .

[213]  S. Villeda,et al.  Mechanisms of Hippocampal Aging and the Potential for Rejuvenation. , 2017, Annual review of neuroscience.

[214]  Tobias Egner,et al.  Monitoring Demands for Executive Control: Shared Functions between Human and Nonhuman Primates , 2017, Trends in Neurosciences.

[215]  G. D. Logan Task Switching , 2022 .