The control of memory-guided reaching movements in peripersonal space.

The goal of the present investigation was to explore the putative contributions of feedforward- and feedback-based processes in the control of memory-guided reaching movements. Participants (N = 4) completed an extensive number of reaching movements (2700) to 3 midline targets (20, 30, 40 cm) in 6 visual conditions: full-vision, open-loop, and four memory-guided conditions (0, 200, 400, and 600 ms of delay). To infer limb control, we used a regression technique to examine the within-trial correspondence between the spatial position of the limb at peak acceleration, peak velocity, peak deceleration, and the ultimate movement endpoint. A high degree of within-trial correspondence would suggest that the final position of the limb was largely specified prior to movement onset and not adjusted during the action (i.e., feedforward control); conversely, a low degree of within-trial correspondence would suggest that movements were modified during the reaching trajectory (i.e., feedback control). Full-vision reaches were found to be more accurate and less variable than open-loop and memory-guided reaches. Moreover, full-vision reaches demonstrated only modest within-trial correspondence between the spatial position of the limb at each kinematic marker and the ultimate movement endpoint, suggesting that reaching accuracy was achieved by adjusting the limb trajectory throughout the course of the action. Open-loop and memory-guided movements exhibited strong within-trial correspondence between final limb position and the position of the limb at peak velocity and peak deceleration. This strong correspondence indicates that the final position of the limb was largely determined by processes that occurred before the reach was initiated; errors in the planning process were not corrected during the course of the action. Thus, and contrary to our previous findings in a video-based aiming task, it appears that stored target information is not extensively (if at all) used to modify the trajectory of reaching movements to remembered targets in peripersonal space.

[1]  Réjean Plamondon,et al.  A kinematic theory of rapid human movements , 1995, Biological Cybernetics.

[2]  D. Elliott,et al.  The influence of visual target and limb information on manual aiming. , 1988, Canadian journal of psychology.

[3]  John F. Kalaska,et al.  Spatial coding of movement: A hypothesis concerning the coding of movement direction by motor cortical populations , 1983 .

[4]  C J Worringham,et al.  Variability effects on the internal structure of rapid aiming movements. , 1991, Journal of motor behavior.

[5]  D. Elliott,et al.  The Influence of Premovement Visual Information on Manual Aiming , 1987, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[6]  Romeo Chua,et al.  Goal-Directed Aiming: Correcting a Force-Specification Error With the Right and Left Hands. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.

[7]  S. Vogt,et al.  Vision of the hand and environmental context in human prehension , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  Francesco Lacquaniti,et al.  Cognitive allocentric representations of visual space shape pointing errors , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  J. T. Massey,et al.  Interruption of motor cortical discharge subserving aimed arm movements , 1983, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  E A Roy,et al.  The accuracy of reaching movements in brief delay conditions. , 2001, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[11]  M. Goodale,et al.  The objects of action and perception , 1998, Cognition.

[12]  R A Abrams,et al.  Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. , 1988, Psychological review.

[13]  D Elliott,et al.  Optimizing the use of Vision in Manual Aiming: The Role of Practice , 1995, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[14]  D. Chaffin,et al.  An investigation of fitts' law using a wide range of movement amplitudes. , 1976, Journal of motor behavior.

[15]  Paul Milgram,et al.  A spectacle-mounted liquid-crystal tachistoscope , 1987 .

[16]  Peter Dixon,et al.  Motor adaptation to an optical illusion , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[17]  John M. Foley,et al.  Error in visually directed manual pointing , 1975 .

[18]  D. Elliott,et al.  The influence of uncertainty and premovement visual information on manual aiming. , 1990, Canadian journal of psychology.

[19]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Comparison of variability of initial kinematics and endpoints of reaching movements , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  P. Fitts The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.

[21]  H. Zelaznik,et al.  Motor-output variability: a theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. , 1979, Psychological review.

[22]  D. Willingham,et al.  Effect of Sex and Joystick Experience on Pursuit Tracking in Adults , 2000, Journal of motor behavior.

[23]  Romeo Chua,et al.  On-line control of rapid aiming movements: Unexpected target perturbations and movement kinematics. , 1998 .

[24]  Réjean Plamondon,et al.  A kinematic theory of rapid human movements , 1995, Biological Cybernetics.

[25]  M. A. Goodale,et al.  Grasping after a Delay Shifts Size-Scaling from Absolute to Relative Metrics , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  Matthew Heath,et al.  The control of goal-directed limb movements: Correcting errors in the trajectory , 1999 .

[27]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Representation of hand position prior to movement and motor variability. , 1995, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[28]  D. Elliott,et al.  A visual representation and the control of manual aiming movements. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.

[29]  D. Elliott,et al.  Visual regulation of manual aiming , 1993 .

[30]  Fred H. Previc,et al.  The Neuropsychology of 3-D Space , 1998 .

[31]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Short-Term Memory for Reaching to Visual Targets: Psychophysical Evidence for Body-Centered Reference Frames , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  C. MacKenzie,et al.  Three-Dimensional Movement Trajectories in Fitts' Task: Implications for Control , 1987 .

[33]  L. Proteau,et al.  The role of peripheral and central visual information for the directional control of manual aiming movements. , 1999, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[34]  M. Goodale,et al.  Perceptual illusion and the real-time control of action. , 2003, Spatial vision.

[35]  Matthew Heath,et al.  The effect of a pictorial illusion on closed-loop and open-loop prehension , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  Les G. Carlton,et al.  Visual Information: The Control of Aiming Movements , 1981 .

[37]  D. Westwood,et al.  No Evidence for Accurate Visuomotor Memory: Systematic and Variable Error in Memory-Guided Reaching , 2003, Journal of motor behavior.

[38]  M. A. Goodale,et al.  The role of visual feedback of hand position in the control of manual prehension , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  M. Bryden Measuring handedness with questionnaires , 1977, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Optimal response of eye and hand motor systems in pointing at a visual target , 1979, Biological Cybernetics.

[41]  R A Abrams,et al.  Rapid aimed limb movements: differential effects of practice on component submovements. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[42]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Viewing the hand prior to movement improves accuracy of pointing performed toward the unseen contralateral hand , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[43]  Digby Elliott,et al.  Rescaling an acquired discrete aiming movement: Specific or general motor learning? , 1997 .

[44]  Lorraine G. Kisselburgh,et al.  Rapid visual feedback processing in single-aiming movements. , 1983, Journal of motor behavior.

[45]  Howard Poizner,et al.  Influence of movement speed on accuracy and coordination of reaching movements to memorized targets in three-dimensional space in a deafferented subject , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[46]  David L. Sparks,et al.  Saccades to remembered target locations: an analysis of systematic and variable errors , 1994, Vision Research.

[47]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Early stages in a sensorimotor transformation , 1992, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[48]  M. Jeannerod The timing of natural prehension movements. , 1984, Journal of motor behavior.

[49]  D. Rosenbaum Human movement initiation: specification of arm, direction, and extent. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[50]  W. Helsen,et al.  A century later: Woodworth's (1899) two-component model of goal-directed aiming. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[51]  Romeo Chua,et al.  Discrete vs. continuous visual control of manual aiming , 1991 .

[52]  M. Lemay,et al.  A distance effect in a manual aiming task to remembered targets: a test of three hypotheses , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[53]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Effect of target size on spatial and temporal characteristics of a pointing movement in man , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[54]  Julie Messier,et al.  Differential effect of task conditions on errors of direction and extent of reaching movements , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[55]  Les G. Carlton,et al.  Chapter 1 Visual Processing Time and the Control of Movement , 1992 .

[56]  Matthew Heath,et al.  Can a visual representation support the online control of memory-dependent reaching? Evident from a variable spatial mapping paradigm. , 2003, Motor control.

[57]  Robert Sessions Woodworth,et al.  THE ACCURACY OF VOLUNTARY MOVEMENT , 1899 .

[58]  James L. Lyons,et al.  Optimal Control Strategies Under Different Feedback Schedules: Kinematic Evidence , 2002, Journal of motor behavior.

[59]  J. Gordon,et al.  Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. II. Effects of visual information on accuracy. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[60]  M F Bradshaw,et al.  The Effect of Pre-Movement Delays on Pointing Accuracy in Middle Childhood , 1998, Perception.

[61]  C Ghez,et al.  Learning of Visuomotor Transformations for Vectorial Planning of Reaching Trajectories , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[62]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Sensorimotor representations for pointing to targets in three-dimensional space. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[63]  D. Elliott,et al.  The role of target information on manual-aiming bias , 1995, Psychological Research.

[64]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement , 1986, Nature.