Multisensory Integration of Looming Signals by Rhesus Monkeys

Looming objects produce ecologically important signals that can be perceived in both the visual and auditory domains. Using a preferential looking technique with looming and receding visual and auditory stimuli, we examined the multisensory integration of looming stimuli by rhesus monkeys. We found a strong attentional preference for coincident visual and auditory looming but no analogous preference for coincident stimulus recession. Consistent with previous findings, the effect occurred only with tonal stimuli and not with broadband noise. The results suggest an evolved capacity to integrate multisensory looming objects.

[1]  A. Walker-Andrews,et al.  Auditory-visual perception of changing distance by human infants. , 1985, Child development.

[2]  Lawrence D. Rosenblum,et al.  Auditory Looming Perception: Influences on Anticipatory Judgments , 1993 .

[3]  R. Desimone,et al.  Visual properties of neurons in a polysensory area in superior temporal sulcus of the macaque. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  Josef P. Rauschecker,et al.  The Development of Intersensory Perception: Comparative Perspectives , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[5]  C. Rowe Receiver psychology and the evolution of multicomponent signals , 1999, Animal Behaviour.

[6]  William Rowan,et al.  The Study of Instinct , 1953 .

[7]  G. Ettlinger,et al.  Cross-modal performance: behavioural processes, phylogenetic considerations and neural mechanisms , 1990, Behavioural Brain Research.

[8]  John G. Neuhoff,et al.  Perceptual bias for rising tones , 1998, Nature.

[9]  D. I. Perrett,et al.  Visual and somatosensory processing in the macaque temporal cortex: the role of ‘expectation’ , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  James A. Caviness,et al.  Persistent Fear Responses in Rhesus Monkeys to the Optical Stimulus of "Looming" , 1962, Science.

[11]  John G. Neuhoff,et al.  An Adaptive Bias in the Perception of Looming Auditory Motion , 2001 .

[12]  J. K. Hietanen,et al.  A comparison of visual responses to object- and ego-motion in the macaque superior temporal polysensory area , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  W Schiff,et al.  Accuracy of judging time to arrival: effects of modality, trajectory, and gender. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  Elizabeth S Spelke,et al.  Perception of Moving, Sounding Objects by Four-Month-Old Infants , 1983, Perception.

[15]  Tirin Moore,et al.  Complex movements evoked by microstimulation of the ventral intraparietal area , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[16]  E R Hafter,et al.  An effect of temporal asymmetry on loudness. , 2000, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[17]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Ventral intraparietal area of the macaque: anatomic location and visual response properties. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  Boris Crassini,et al.  Use of an auditory looming task to test infants’ sensitivity to sound pressure level as an auditory distance cue , 2001 .

[19]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[20]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Neuroperception: Facial expressions linked to monkey calls , 2003, Nature.

[21]  Albert S. Bregman,et al.  The Auditory Scene. (Book Reviews: Auditory Scene Analysis. The Perceptual Organization of Sound.) , 1990 .

[22]  L. Rosenblum,et al.  Relative Effectiveness of Three Stimulus Variables for Locating a Moving Sound Source , 1987, Perception.

[23]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Polysensory properties of neurons in the anterior bank of the caudal superior temporal sulcus of the macaque monkey. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  Lawrence G. McDade,et al.  Behavioral Indices of Multisensory Integration: Orientation to Visual Cues is Affected by Auditory Stimuli , 1989, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[25]  R. Desimone,et al.  Visual areas in the temporal cortex of the macaque , 1979, Brain Research.

[26]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  The bimodal perception of speech in infancy. , 1982, Science.

[27]  R. Seyfarth,et al.  Loud calls as indicators of dominance in male baboons (Papio cynocephalus ursinus) , 2003, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[28]  François Klam,et al.  ã Federation of European Neuroscience Societies Visual±vestibular interactive responses in the macaque ventral intraparietal area (VIP) , 2022 .

[29]  N. Tinbergen,et al.  The Study of Instinct , 1953 .

[30]  W. Schiff PERCEPTION OF IMPENDING COLLISION: A STUDY OF VISUALLY DIRECTED AVOIDANT BEHAVIOR. , 1965, Psychological monographs.

[31]  Nikos K. Logothetis,et al.  Auditory looming perception in rhesus monkeys , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[32]  John G. Neuhoff,et al.  Neural Processing of Auditory Looming in the Human Brain , 2002, Current Biology.

[33]  Janeene M. Touchton,et al.  Rapid acquisition of an alarm response by a neotropical primate to a newly introduced avian predator , 2003, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[34]  K. Zilles,et al.  Polymodal Motion Processing in Posterior Parietal and Premotor Cortex A Human fMRI Study Strongly Implies Equivalencies between Humans and Monkeys , 2001, Neuron.

[35]  D H Ashmead,et al.  Contribution of listeners' approaching motion to auditory distance perception. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[36]  W. Ball,et al.  Infant Responses to Impending Collision: Optical and Real , 1971, Science.

[37]  Ralph M. Siegel,et al.  Optic Flow Selectivity in the Anterior Superior Temporal Polysensory Area, STPa, of the Behaving Monkey , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.