Cortical mechanism of communicative speech production

Communicative speech requires conformity not only to linguistic rules but also to behavior that is appropriate for social interaction. The existence of a special brain mechanism for such behavioral aspects of communicative speech has been suggested by studies of social impairment in autism, and it may be related to communicative vocalization in animals. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to measure cortical activation while normal subjects casually talked to an actor (communication task) or verbally described a situation (description task) while observing video clips of an action performed by a familiar or an unfamiliar actor in a typical daily situation. We assumed that the communication task differed from the description task in the involvement of behavioral aspects of communicative speech production, which may involve the processing of interaction-relevant biographical information. Significantly higher activation was observed during the communication task than during the description task in the medial prefrontal cortex (polar and dorsal parts), the bilateral anterior superior temporal sulci, and the left temporoparietal junction. The results suggest that these regions play a role in the behavioral aspects of communicative speech production, presumably in understanding of the context of the social interaction. The activation of the polar part of the medial prefrontal cortex during the communication task was greater when the actor was familiar than when the actor was unfamiliar, suggesting that this region is involved in communicative speech production with reference to biological information. The precuneus was activated during the communication task only with the familiar actor, suggesting that this region is related to access to biographical information per se.

[1]  Derek Edwards,et al.  Discourse, cognition and social practices: the rich surface of language and social interaction , 2006 .

[2]  R. Veit,et al.  Differential cerebral activation during observation of expressive gestures and motor acts , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  The Mind's Eye—Precuneus Activation in Memory-Related Imagery , 1995, NeuroImage.

[4]  T. Hendler,et al.  The Neural Reality of Syntactic Transformations , 2003, Psychological science.

[5]  K. A. Hadland,et al.  The anterior cingulate and reward-guided selection of actions. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  Andreas Schulze-Bonhage,et al.  Precuneus is involved in allocentric spatial location encoding and recognition , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  C. Frith,et al.  “Hey John”: Signals Conveying Communicative Intention toward the Self Activate Brain Regions Associated with “Mentalizing,” Regardless of Modality , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  Hsuan-Chih Chen,et al.  Brain Responses to Segmentally and Tonally Induced Semantic Violations in Cantonese , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[9]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  The evolution of the language faculty: Clarifications and implications , 2005, Cognition.

[10]  C. Frith,et al.  Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  T. Allison,et al.  Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[12]  S. Baron-Cohen Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind , 1997 .

[13]  Isabell Wartenburger,et al.  Neural correlates of syntactic transformations , 2004, Human brain mapping.

[14]  M. Hauser,et al.  Functional referents and acoustic similarity: field playback experiments with rhesus monkeys , 1998, Animal Behaviour.

[15]  Kenneth Mark Colby,et al.  Clinical artificial intelligence , 1981, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[16]  T. Shallice,et al.  Right prefrontal cortex and episodic memory retrieval: a functional MRI test of the monitoring hypothesis. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[17]  M. Rugg,et al.  Separating the Brain Regions Involved in Recollection and Familiarity in Recognition Memory , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  Istvan Molnar-Szakacs,et al.  Watching social interactions produces dorsomedial prefrontal and medial parietal BOLD fMRI signal increases compared to a resting baseline , 2004, NeuroImage.

[19]  R. Saxe Uniquely human social cognition , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[20]  C. Frith,et al.  Interacting minds--a biological basis. , 1999, Science.

[21]  P Hagoort,et al.  A neural correlate of syntactic encoding during speech production , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[22]  M. Goldstein,et al.  Social interaction shapes babbling: Testing parallels between birdsong and speech , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[23]  K. Willmes,et al.  Spatial attention: more than intrinsic alerting? , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  Marc D. Hauser,et al.  The Neurophysiology of Functionally Meaningful Categories: Macaque Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex Plays a Critical Role in Spontaneous Categorization of Species-Specific Vocalizations , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[25]  A R McIntosh,et al.  General and specific brain regions involved in encoding and retrieval of events: what, where, and when. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[26]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? , 2002, Science.

[27]  Uwe Jürgens,et al.  Afferents of vocalization-controlling periaqueductal regions in the squirrel monkey , 2005, Brain Research.

[28]  S Kuriki,et al.  Motor planning center for speech articulation in the normal human brain. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[29]  R. Mar The neuropsychology of narrative: story comprehension, story production and their interrelation , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[30]  E. Tulving,et al.  Novelty encoding networks in the human brain: positron emission tomography data. , 1994, Neuroreport.

[31]  David I. Perrett,et al.  An fMRI study of joint attention experience , 2005, NeuroImage.

[32]  A. Braun,et al.  Toward an evolutionary perspective on conceptual representation: species-specific calls activate visual and affective processing systems in the macaque. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[33]  Nadim Joni Shah,et al.  Cortical Representations of Personally Familiar Objects and Places: Functional Organization of the Human Posterior Cingulate Cortex , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[34]  Henrik Walter,et al.  Understanding Intentions in Social Interaction: The Role of the Anterior Paracingulate Cortex , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[35]  Allen R. Braun,et al.  Brain network interactions in auditory, visual and linguistic processing , 2004, Brain and Language.

[36]  C. Frith,et al.  Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’ , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[37]  S. Pinker,et al.  The faculty of language: what's special about it? , 2005, Cognition.

[38]  C. Koch,et al.  Brain Areas Specific for Attentional Load in a Motion-Tracking Task , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[39]  F. Inglis How To Do Things With Words. , 1971 .

[40]  Masatoshi Itoh,et al.  Target dependency of brain mechanism involved in dispositional inference: a PET study , 2004, NeuroImage.

[41]  James K Rilling,et al.  The neural correlates of theory of mind within interpersonal interactions , 2004, NeuroImage.

[42]  P. Hagoort On Broca, brain, and binding: a new framework , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[43]  P. Mundy,et al.  On the nature of communication and language impairment in autism , 1997 .

[44]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A Common Network of Functional Areas for Attention and Eye Movements , 1998, Neuron.

[45]  T. Uema,et al.  Abnormal regional cerebral blood flow in childhood autism. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[46]  Catherine Adams,et al.  Conversational behaviour of children with Asperger syndrome and conduct disorder. , 2002, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[47]  C. Frith,et al.  Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[48]  S. Carey,et al.  Understanding other minds: linking developmental psychology and functional neuroimaging. , 2004, Annual review of psychology.

[49]  C Büchel,et al.  Brain regions involved in articulation , 1999, The Lancet.

[50]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[51]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[52]  J. Flavell Development of children’s knowledge about the mental world , 2000 .

[53]  Cameron S Carter,et al.  Prefrontal Cortex Guides Context-Appropriate Responding during Language Production , 2004, Neuron.

[54]  J. Austin How to do things with words , 1962 .