Orientation tuning of contrast masking caused by motion streaks.

We investigated whether the oriented trails of blur left by fast-moving dots (i.e., "motion streaks") effectively mask grating targets. Using a classic overlay masking paradigm, we varied mask contrast and target orientation to reveal underlying tuning. Fast-moving Gaussian blob arrays elevated thresholds for detection of static gratings, both monoptically and dichoptically. Monoptic masking at high mask (i.e., streak) contrasts is tuned for orientation and exhibits a similar bandwidth to masking functions obtained with grating stimuli (∼30 degrees). Dichoptic masking fails to show reliable orientation-tuned masking, but dichoptic masks at very low contrast produce a narrowly tuned facilitation (∼17 degrees). For iso-oriented streak masks and grating targets, we also explored masking as a function of mask contrast. Interestingly, dichoptic masking shows a classic "dipper"-like TVC function, whereas monoptic masking shows no dip and a steeper "handle". There is a very strong unoriented component to the masking, which we attribute to transiently biased temporal frequency masking. Fourier analysis of "motion streak" images shows interesting differences between dichoptic and monoptic functions and the information in the stimulus. Our data add weight to the growing body of evidence that the oriented blur of motion streaks contributes to the processing of fast motion signals.

[1]  H. Barlow Temporal and spatial summation in human vision at different background intensities , 1958, The Journal of physiology.

[2]  D. H. Kelly Visual response to time-dependent stimuli. I. Amplitude sensitivity measurements. , 1961, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[3]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex , 1962, The Journal of physiology.

[4]  F. Campbell,et al.  Orientational selectivity of the human visual system , 1966, The Journal of physiology.

[5]  J. Roufs Dynamic properties of vision. I. Experimental relationships between flicker and flash thresholds. , 1972, Vision research.

[6]  J. Roufs Dynamic properties of vision. IV. Thresholds of decremental flashes, incremental flashes and doublets in relation to flicker fusion. , 1974, Vision research.

[7]  A. Watson Probability summation over time , 1979, Vision Research.

[8]  D. Burr Temporal summation of moving images by the human visual system , 1981, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[9]  R. L. Valois,et al.  The orientation and direction selectivity of cells in macaque visual cortex , 1982, Vision Research.

[10]  A. Watson,et al.  Quest: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[11]  H. Wilson,et al.  Orientation bandwidths of spatial mechanisms measured by masking. , 1984, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[12]  Randolph Blake,et al.  Orientation selectivity in cats and humans assessed by masking , 1985, Vision Research.

[13]  D. Pollen,et al.  Spatial and temporal frequency selectivity of neurones in visual cortical areas V1 and V2 of the macaque monkey. , 1985, The Journal of physiology.

[14]  E H Adelson,et al.  Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[15]  A J Ahumada,et al.  Model of human visual-motion sensing. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[16]  J. van Santen,et al.  Elaborated Reichardt detectors. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[17]  Mark A. Georgeson,et al.  Temporal properties of spatial contrast vision , 1987, Vision Research.

[18]  A. B. Bonds Role of Inhibition in the Specification of Orientation Selectivity of Cells in the Cat Striate Cortex , 1989, Visual Neuroscience.

[19]  R. Snowden Measurement of visual channels by contrast adaptation , 1991, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[20]  J. Ross,et al.  Contrast adaptation and contrast masking in human vision , 1991, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[21]  O. Braddick,et al.  The temporal integration and resolution of velocity signals , 1991, Vision Research.

[22]  D. Heeger Normalization of cell responses in cat striate cortex , 1992, Visual Neuroscience.

[23]  L K Cormack,et al.  Disparity-tuned channels of the human visual system , 1993, Visual Neuroscience.

[24]  G. Westheimer,et al.  Discrimination of direction of motion in human vision. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  J. Movshon,et al.  Linearity and Normalization in Simple Cells of the Macaque Primary Visual Cortex , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[27]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[28]  R. Hess,et al.  On the relationship between the spatial channels for luminance and disparity processing , 1999, Vision Research.

[29]  Wilson S. Geisler,et al.  Motion streaks provide a spatial code for motion direction , 1999, Nature.

[30]  J. M. Foley,et al.  Temporal sensitivity of human luminance pattern mechanisms determined by masking with temporally modulated stimuli , 1999, Vision Research.

[31]  David R. Badcock,et al.  Coherent global motion in the absence of coherent velocity signals , 2000, Current Biology.

[32]  D. G. Albrecht,et al.  Motion direction signals in the primary visual cortex of cat and monkey. , 2001, Visual neuroscience.

[33]  A. B. Bonds,et al.  Temporal-frequency tuning of cross-orientation suppression in the cat striate cortex , 2001, Visual Neuroscience.

[34]  John Ross,et al.  Direct Evidence That “Speedlines” Influence Motion Mechanisms , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  Nicholas J Priebe,et al.  A New Mechanism for Neuronal Gain Control (or How the Gain in Brains Has Mainly Been Explained) , 2002, Neuron.

[36]  M. Carandini,et al.  Masking by fast gratings. , 2002, Journal of vision.

[37]  M. Carandini,et al.  Suppression without Inhibition in Visual Cortex , 2002, Neuron.

[38]  Frank Bremmer,et al.  Neural correlates of implied motion , 2003, Nature.

[39]  C. Stromeyer,et al.  Human temporal impulse response speeds up with increased stimulus contrast , 2003, Vision Research.

[40]  M. C. Morrone,et al.  Cross-orientation inhibition in cat is GABA mediated , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[41]  Justin A. Harris,et al.  Contextual Modulation outside of Awareness , 2005, Current Biology.

[42]  P. Hibbard The orientation bandwidth of cyclopean channels , 2005, Vision Research.

[43]  G. Maehara,et al.  Binocular, Monocular and Dichoptic Pattern Masking , 2005 .

[44]  Moshe Gur,et al.  Cerebral Cortex doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi003 Orientation and Direction Selectivity of Neurons in V1 of Alert Monkeys: Functional Relationships and Laminar Distributions , 2022 .

[45]  M. Georgeson,et al.  Binocular contrast vision at and above threshold. , 2006, Journal of vision.

[46]  T. Meese,et al.  Spatial and temporal dependencies of cross-orientation suppression in human vision , 2007, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[47]  David Fitzpatrick,et al.  Luminance-Evoked Inhibition in Primary Visual Cortex: A Transient Veto of Simultaneous and Ongoing Response , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  D. Alais,et al.  Evidence for two interacting temporal channels in human visual processing , 2006, Vision Research.

[49]  T. Meese,et al.  Binocular contrast interactions: Dichoptic masking is not a single process , 2007, Vision Research.

[50]  Jeroen J. A. van Boxtel,et al.  Dichoptic masking and binocular rivalry share common perceptual dynamics. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[52]  Mark Edwards,et al.  Motion streaks improve motion detection , 2007, Vision Research.

[53]  D. Alais,et al.  Tilt aftereffects and tilt illusions induced by fast translational motion: evidence for motion streaks. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[54]  T. Meese,et al.  Cross-orientation masking is speed invariant between ocular pathways but speed dependent within them. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[55]  David Alais,et al.  Temporal whitening: transient noise perceptually equalizes the 1/f temporal amplitude spectrum. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[56]  Sean P. MacEvoy,et al.  A precise form of divisive suppression supports population coding in primary visual cortex , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[57]  David Alais,et al.  Motion streaks in fast motion rivalry cause orientation-selective suppression. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[58]  D. Alais,et al.  Orientation bandwidths are invariant across spatiotemporal frequency after isotropic components are removed. , 2009, Journal of vision.