Neural Computations Governing Spatiotemporal Pooling of Visual Motion Signals in Humans

The brain estimates visual motion by decoding the responses of populations of neurons. Extracting unbiased motion estimates from early visual cortical neurons is challenging because each neuron contributes an ambiguous (local) representation of the visual environment and inherently variable neural response. To mitigate these sources of noise, the brain can pool across large populations of neurons, pool the response of each neuron over time, or a combination of the two. Recent psychophysical and physiological work points to a flexible motion pooling system that arrives at different computational solutions over time and for different stimuli. Here we ask whether a single, likelihood-based computation can accommodate the flexible nature of spatiotemporal motion pooling in humans. We examined the contribution of different computations to human observers' performance on two global visual motion discriminations tasks, one requiring the combination of motion directions over time and another requiring their combination in different relative proportions over space and time. Observers' perceived direction of global motion was accurately predicted by a vector average readout of direction signals accumulated over time and a maximum likelihood readout of direction signals combined over space, consistent with the notion of a flexible motion pooling system that uses different computations over space and time. Additional simulations of observers' performance with a population decoding model revealed a more parsimonious solution: flexible spatiotemporal pooling could be accommodated by a single computation that optimally pools motion signals across a population of neurons that accumulate local motion signals on their receptive fields at a fixed rate over time.

[1]  A. Pouget,et al.  Reading population codes: a neural implementation of ideal observers , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[2]  H Sompolinsky,et al.  Simple models for reading neuronal population codes. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[3]  Scott N. J. Watamaniuk,et al.  Temporal and spatial integration in dynamic random-dot stimuli , 1992, Vision Research.

[4]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Temporal integration of random dot apparent motion information in human central vision , 1994, Vision Research.

[5]  G. Orban,et al.  Human velocity and direction discrimination measured with random dot patterns , 1988, Vision Research.

[6]  S. Nishida,et al.  Adaptive pooling of visual motion signals by the human visual system revealed with a novel multi-element stimulus. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[7]  D. Bradley,et al.  Structure and function of visual area MT. , 2005, Annual review of neuroscience.

[8]  D. G. Albrecht,et al.  Motion selectivity and the contrast-response function of simple cells in the visual cortex , 1991, Visual Neuroscience.

[9]  Robert Sekuler,et al.  Coherent global motion percepts from stochastic local motions , 1984, Vision Research.

[10]  P. Wenderoth,et al.  The effect of interactions between one-dimensional component gratings on two-dimensional motion perception , 1993, Vision Research.

[11]  J. van Santen,et al.  Temporal covariance model of human motion perception. , 1984, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[12]  M. Shiffrar,et al.  Different motion sensitive units are involved in recovering the direction of moving lines , 1993, Vision Research.

[13]  Andrew B. Watson,et al.  A look at motion in the frequency domain , 1983 .

[14]  O. Braddick,et al.  Integration across Directions in Dynamic Random Dot Displays: Vector Summation or Winner Take All? , 1996, Vision Research.

[15]  David C. Burr,et al.  Seeing biological motion , 1998, Nature.

[16]  P. Thompson,et al.  Human speed perception is contrast dependent , 1992, Vision Research.

[17]  Anthony J. Movshon,et al.  Optimal representation of sensory information by neural populations , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[18]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex , 1962, The Journal of physiology.

[19]  V. Ferrera Task-dependent modulation of the sensorimotor transformation for smooth pursuit eye movements. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  S. Treue,et al.  The response of neurons in areas V1 and MT of the alert rhesus monkey to moving random dot patterns , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  M. A. Smith,et al.  Stimulus Dependence of Neuronal Correlation in Primary Visual Cortex of the Macaque , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  M. Paradiso,et al.  A theory for the use of visual orientation information which exploits the columnar structure of striate cortex , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[23]  T. Albright,et al.  Efficient Discrimination of Temporal Patterns by Motion-Sensitive Neurons in Primate Visual Cortex , 1998, Neuron.

[24]  David R. Badcock,et al.  On the role of second-order signals in the perceived direction of motion of Type II plaid patterns , 1994, Vision Research.

[25]  O. Braddick,et al.  The temporal integration and resolution of velocity signals , 1991, Vision Research.

[26]  J. Movshon,et al.  Motion Integration by Neurons in Macaque MT Is Local, Not Global , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  T. Albright Direction and orientation selectivity of neurons in visual area MT of the macaque. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  How MT cells analyze the motion of visual patterns , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[29]  J. J. Koenderink,et al.  Temporal properties of the visual detectability of moving spatial white noise , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  Christopher C. Pack,et al.  Temporal dynamics of a neural solution to the aperture problem in visual area MT of macaque brain , 2001, Nature.

[31]  J. Raymond Movement direction analysers: Independence and bandwidth , 1993, Vision Research.

[32]  J. Anthony Movshon,et al.  Dynamics of Pattern Motion Computation , 2009 .

[33]  Guillaume S Masson,et al.  Temporal dynamics of 2D motion integration for ocular following in macaque monkeys. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  O. Braddick Segmentation versus integration in visual motion processing , 1993, Trends in Neurosciences.

[35]  R A Andersen,et al.  The response of area MT and V1 neurons to transparent motion , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[36]  D. Badcock,et al.  Global motion perception: No interaction between the first- and second-order motion pathways , 1995, Vision Research.

[37]  A. Vassilev,et al.  Perception time and spatial frequency , 1976, Vision Research.

[38]  P. Thompson Perceived rate of movement depends on contrast , 1982, Vision Research.

[39]  Nicholas J. Priebe,et al.  Constraints on the source of short-term motion adaptation in macaque area MT. I. the role of input and intrinsic mechanisms. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  Hugh R. Wilson,et al.  Perceived direction of moving two-dimensional patterns depends on duration, contrast and eccentricity , 1992, Vision Research.

[41]  Christopher C. Pack,et al.  Temporal Dynamics of Motion Integration , 2009 .

[42]  T. Sanger,et al.  Probability density estimation for the interpretation of neural population codes. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  G. Boynton,et al.  The invariance of directional tuning with contrast and coherence , 2004, Vision Research.

[44]  J. Movshon,et al.  Dynamics of motion signaling by neurons in macaque area MT , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[45]  Scott N. J. Watamaniuk,et al.  Direction Perception in Complex Dynamic Displays: the Integration of Dir~~tion Information , 1988 .

[46]  P. Földiák,et al.  The ‘Ideal Homunculus’: Statistical Inference from Neural Population Responses , 1993 .

[47]  E H Adelson,et al.  Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[48]  William Bialek,et al.  Reading a Neural Code , 1991, NIPS.

[49]  J. Nachmias Effect of Exposure Duration on Visual Contrast Sensitivity with Square-Wave Gratings*† , 1967 .

[50]  Xin Huang,et al.  Noise correlations in cortical area MT and their potential impact on trial-by-trial variation in the direction and speed of smooth-pursuit eye movements. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[51]  D. Regan,et al.  Postadaptation orientation discrimination. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[52]  D. Burr,et al.  The Ventriloquist Effect Results from Near-Optimal Bimodal Integration , 2004, Current Biology.

[53]  A. Dean The variability of discharge of simple cells in the cat striate cortex , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[54]  B. Finlay,et al.  Short-term response variability of monkey striate neurons , 1976, Brain Research.

[55]  Steven C. Dakin,et al.  Local and global limitations on direction integration assessed using equivalent noise analysis , 2005, Vision Research.

[56]  R. Jacobs,et al.  Optimal integration of texture and motion cues to depth , 1999, Vision Research.

[57]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  Noise characteristics and prior expectations in human visual speed perception , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[58]  Guillaume S. Masson,et al.  From 1D to 2D via 3D: dynamics of surface motion segmentation for ocular tracking in primates , 2004, Journal of Physiology-Paris.

[59]  W. Bair,et al.  Correlated Firing in Macaque Visual Area MT: Time Scales and Relationship to Behavior , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[60]  M. Ernst,et al.  Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.

[61]  M. Landy,et al.  Measurement and modeling of depth cue combination: in defense of weak fusion , 1995, Vision Research.

[62]  Ennio Mingolla,et al.  Neural models of motion integration and segmentation , 2003, Neural Networks.

[63]  D. Burr,et al.  Temporal integration of optic flow, measured by contrast and coherence thresholds , 2001, Vision Research.

[64]  A J Ahumada,et al.  Model of human visual-motion sensing. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[65]  J. Movshon,et al.  A computational analysis of the relationship between neuronal and behavioral responses to visual motion , 1996, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[66]  K. H. Britten,et al.  Responses of neurons in macaque MT to stochastic motion signals , 1993, Visual Neuroscience.

[67]  Timothy Ledgeway,et al.  Cortical pooling algorithms for judging global motion direction , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[68]  D J Heeger,et al.  Model for the extraction of image flow. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[69]  Carlos R. Ponce,et al.  Temporal evolution of 2-dimensional direction signals used to guide eye movements. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[70]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Coding of image contrast in central visual pathways of the macaque monkey , 1990, Vision Research.

[71]  Robert Sekuler,et al.  A two-dimensional analysis of direction-specific adaptation , 1980, Vision Research.

[72]  Edward H. Adelson,et al.  Motion illusions as optimal percepts , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[73]  W T Newsome,et al.  Tuning bandwidths for near-threshold stimuli in area MT. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[74]  Christopher C. Pack,et al.  The role of V1 surround suppression in MT motion integration. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[75]  G H Recanzone,et al.  Shift in smooth pursuit initiation and MT and MST neuronal activity under different stimulus conditions. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[76]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  A model of neuronal responses in visual area MT , 1998, Vision Research.

[77]  D. J. Felleman,et al.  Receptive-field properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area (MT) of owl monkeys. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.

[78]  J. Movshon,et al.  Adaptive Temporal Integration of Motion in Direction-Selective Neurons in Macaque Visual Cortex , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[79]  Jonathan W. Peirce,et al.  PsychoPy—Psychophysics software in Python , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[80]  Christopher C. Pack,et al.  Dynamic properties of neurons in cortical area MT in alert and anaesthetized macaque monkeys , 2001, Nature.

[81]  J. B. Mulligan,et al.  Effect of contrast on the perceived direction of a moving plaid , 1990, Vision Research.

[82]  L. Bowns Evidence for a Feature Tracking Explanation of Why Type II Plaids Move in the Vector Sum Direction at Short Durations , 1996, Vision Research.

[83]  Ehud Zohary,et al.  Correlated neuronal discharge rate and its implications for psychophysical performance , 1994, Nature.