The role of abstract syntactic knowledge in language acquisition: a reply to Tomasello (2000)

In his paper "Do young children have adult syntactic competence?" Tomasello (Cognition 74 (2000) 209) interprets young children's conservatism in language production as evidence that early language use, and verb use in particular, are based entirely on concrete lexical representations, showing no evidence of abstract syntactic categories such as "verb" or "transitive sentence". In this reply, I argue that Tomasello's interpretation depends on three questionable premises: (a) that anyone with a robust grammatical category of verbs would use new verbs in unattested sentence constructions; (b) that there are no reasons other than lack of syntactic competence for lexical effects in language use; and (c) that children always interpret a new verb presented in the context of an action on an object as a causal action verb, and therefore as one they should use transitively. I review evidence against all of these assumptions. Tomasello's data, among others', show that children indeed learn item-specific facts about verbs and other lexical items - as they must, to become competent speakers of their native language. However, other data suggest that more abstract descriptions of linguistic input also play a role in early language use. To achieve a complete picture of how children learn their native languages, we must explore the interactions of lexical and more abstract syntactic knowledge in language acquisition.

[1]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Fourteen-through 18-month-old infants di eren-tially imitate intentional and accidental actions , 1998 .

[2]  M. H. Kelly,et al.  Domain-general abilities applied to domain-specific tasks: Sensitivity to probabilities in perception, cognition, and language☆ , 1994 .

[3]  R. Schreuder,et al.  From concepts to lexical items , 1992, Cognition.

[4]  M. Braine,et al.  Can children use a verb without exposure to its argument structure? , 1990, Journal of Child Language.

[5]  M. Tomasello Do young children have adult syntactic competence? , 2000, Cognition.

[6]  Jill G. DeVilliers Learning How to Use Verbs: Lexical Coding and the Influence of the Input. , 1985 .

[7]  R N Aslin,et al.  Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996, Science.

[8]  G. Miller,et al.  Linguistic theory and psychological reality , 1982 .

[9]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Early referential understanding: Infants' ability to recognize referential acts for what they are. , 1993 .

[10]  R. Gómez,et al.  Artificial grammar learning by 1-year-olds leads to specific and abstract knowledge , 1999, Cognition.

[11]  Ellen Bialystok,et al.  Native language and foreign language acquisition , 1981 .

[12]  M. Tomasello First Verbs: A Case Study of Early Grammatical Development , 1994 .

[13]  S. Levinson,et al.  Language Acquisition and Conceptual Development , 2001 .

[14]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Twenty-Five-Month-Old Children Do Not Have a Grammatical Category of Verb. , 1993 .

[15]  J. Trueswell,et al.  How to Prune a Garden Path by Nipping It in the Bud: Fast Priming of Verb Argument Structure , 1998 .

[16]  Elissa L Newport,et al.  Structural packaging in the input to language learning: Contributions of prosodic and morphological marking of phrases to the acquisition of language , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.

[17]  Alexandra A. Cleland,et al.  Syntactic co-ordination in dialogue , 2000, Cognition.

[18]  Stan A. Kuczaj,et al.  Children's Use of the Wh Question Modal Auxiliary Placement Rule. , 1979 .

[19]  W. Bruce Croft Typology and Universals , 1990 .

[20]  M. Maratsos Commentary: relations of lexical specificity to general categories , 1998 .

[21]  Rachel I. Mayberry,et al.  The long-lasting advantage of learning sign language in childhood: Another look at the critical period for language acquisition , 1991 .

[22]  Letitia R. Naigles,et al.  Children use syntax to learn verb meanings , 1990, Journal of Child Language.

[23]  H. Gleitman,et al.  Linguistics: The Cambridge Survey: Where learning begins: initial representations for language learning , 1988 .

[24]  Cynthia L Fisher,et al.  Structural limits on verb mapping: the role of abstract structure in 2.5‐year‐olds’ interpretations of novel verbs , 2002 .

[25]  K. Bock,et al.  From conceptual roles to structural relations: bridging the syntactic cleft. , 1992 .

[26]  Jill de Villiers,et al.  Learning how to use verbs: lexical coding and the influence of the input , 1985, Journal of Child Language.

[27]  J. Musolino,et al.  Children's command of quantification , 2002, Cognition.

[28]  E. Markman,et al.  When it is better to receive than to give: Syntactic and conceptual constraints on vocabulary growth , 1994 .

[29]  A. Senghas,et al.  Children Creating Language: How Nicaraguan Sign Language Acquired a Spatial Grammar , 2001, Psychological science.

[30]  Gary F. Marcus,et al.  German Inflection: The Exception That Proves the Rule , 1995, Cognitive Psychology.

[31]  Elissa L. Newport,et al.  Maturational Constraints on Language Learning , 1990, Cogn. Sci..

[32]  E. Kako,et al.  First contact in verb acquisition: defining a role for syntax. , 1993, Child development.

[33]  Steven Pinker,et al.  Language learnability and language development , 1985 .

[34]  L. Abbeduto Language Development: From Two to Three. , 1992 .

[35]  Dedre Gentner,et al.  Why Nouns Are Learned before Verbs: Linguistic Relativity Versus Natural Partitioning. Technical Report No. 257. , 1982 .

[36]  H. Gleitman,et al.  Human simulations of vocabulary learning , 1999, Cognition.

[37]  M. Maratsos Are actions to verbs as objects are to nouns? On the differential semantic bases of form, class, category , 1990 .

[38]  L. Gleitman,et al.  Language and Experience: Evidence from the Blind Child , 1988 .

[39]  C. Fisher Structural Limits on Verb Mapping: The Role of Analogy in Children's Interpretations of Sentences , 1996, Cognitive Psychology.

[40]  D. Gentner,et al.  Language acquisition and conceptual development: Individuation, relativity, and early word learning , 2001 .

[41]  Letitia R. Naigles,et al.  Learnability and Cognition: The Acquisition of Argument Structure , 1991 .

[42]  MICHAEL TOMASELLO,et al.  Young children's earliest transitive and intransitive constructions , 1998 .

[43]  Letitia R. Naigles,et al.  Input to verb learning: Evidence for the plausibility of syntactic bootstrapping , 1995 .

[44]  Beth Levin,et al.  English Verb Classes and Alternations: A Preliminary Investigation , 1993 .

[45]  H. Pashler STEVENS' HANDBOOK OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY , 2002 .

[46]  B. Levin Unaccusativity: At the Syntax-Lexical Semantics Interface , 1994 .

[47]  Sandra R. Waxman,et al.  Words as Invitations to Form Categories: Evidence from 12- to 13-Month-Old Infants , 1995, Cognitive Psychology.

[48]  L. Gleitman The Structural Sources of Verb Meanings , 2020, Sentence First, Arguments Afterward.

[49]  Susan M. Garnsey,et al.  The Contributions of Verb Bias and Plausibility to the Comprehension of Temporarily Ambiguous Sentences , 1997 .

[50]  S. Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Spontaneous sign systems created by deaf children in two cultures , 1998, Nature.

[51]  Melissa Bowerman THE CHILD'S EXPRESSION OF MEANING: EXPANDING RELATIONSHIPS AMONG LEXICON, SYNTAX, AND MORPHOLOGY * , 1981 .

[52]  I. M. Schlesinger,et al.  Categories and Processes in Language Acquisition , 1990 .

[53]  Steven Pinker,et al.  Productivity and constraints in the acquisition of the passive , 1987, Cognition.

[54]  Michael P Maratsos New models in language and language acquisition , 1978 .

[55]  A. Goldberg Constructions: A Construction Grammar Approach to Argument Structure , 1995 .

[56]  N. Akhtar,et al.  Acquiring basic word order: evidence for data-driven learning of syntactic structure , 1999, Journal of Child Language.

[57]  M. Bowerman Mapping thematic roles onto syntactic functions: are children helped by innate linking rules? , 1990 .

[58]  C. Fillmore The case for case reopened , 1977 .

[59]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Young children learn to produce passives with nonce verbs. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[60]  M. Pickering,et al.  The Representation of Verbs: Evidence from Syntactic Priming in Language Production , 1998 .

[61]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Establishing word-object relations: a first step. , 1989, Child development.

[62]  Cynthia L Fisher,et al.  BREAKING THE LINGUISTIC CODE: CURRENT ISSUES IN EARLY LANGUAGE LEARNING , 2002 .

[63]  Irina A. Sekerina,et al.  The kindergarten-path effect: studying on-line sentence processing in young children , 1999, Cognition.

[64]  David R. Dowty Thematic proto-roles and argument selection , 1991 .

[65]  Leonard Talmy,et al.  (1) Lexicalization patterns: Semantic structure in lexical forms; and , 1987 .

[66]  Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,et al.  The Origins of Grammar: Evidence from Early Language Comprehension , 1999 .

[67]  C. L. Baker,et al.  The Logical problem of language acquisition , 1984 .

[68]  J. Chafetz,et al.  Verb-based versus class-based accounts of actionality effects in children's comprehension of passives , 1990, Cognition.

[69]  M. Braine What sort of innate structure is needed to “bootstrap” into syntax? , 1992, Cognition.

[70]  A. Meltzoff Understanding the Intentions of Others: Re-Enactment of Intended Acts by 18-Month-Old Children. , 1995, Developmental psychology.

[71]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Joint attention and early language. , 1986, Child development.

[72]  L. Bloom Language Development: Form and Function in Emerging Grammars , 1970 .

[73]  L. Bloom,et al.  Language development from two to three , 1995 .