Learning to walk changes infants' social interactions.

[1]  I. Sigel,et al.  HANDBOOK OF CHILD PSYCHOLOGY , 2006 .

[2]  V. Denenberg,et al.  Early experience and emotional development. , 1963, Scientific American.

[3]  H. L. Rheingold,et al.  Maternal behavior in mammals. , 1963 .

[4]  I. Philips The Psychological Birth of the Human Infant , 1976 .

[5]  Dale F. Hay,et al.  Sharing in the second year of life. , 1976 .

[6]  L. Acredolo Development of Spatial Orientation in Infancy. , 1978 .

[7]  M. West,et al.  Effects of infant development on mother-infant interactions. , 1980, Child development.

[8]  J. Richards,et al.  On the nature of the visual-cliff-avoidance response in human infants. , 1980, Child development.

[9]  J. Richards,et al.  Crawling-onset age predicts visual cliff avoidance in infants. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  C. Rovee-Collier,et al.  Advances in infancy research , 1981 .

[11]  G. Gustafson,et al.  Effects of the ability to locomote on infants''social and exploratory behaviors , 1984 .

[12]  E J Gibson,et al.  Detection of the traversability of surfaces by crawling and walking infants. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  R. Kermoian,et al.  Locomotor Experience: A Facilitator of Spatial Cognitive Development, , 1988, Child development.

[14]  E. Gibson Exploratory behavior in the development of perceiving, acting, and the acquiring of knowledge. , 1988 .

[15]  Connie Kasari,et al.  Nonverbal communication, affective sharing, and intersubjectivity , 1992 .

[16]  J. Campos,et al.  Socioemotional transformations in the family system following infant crawling onset. , 1992, New directions for child development.

[17]  Nancy Eisenberg,et al.  Emotion and Its Regulation in Early Development , 1992 .

[18]  Bennett I. Bertenthal,et al.  Early Experience and Emotional Development: The Emergence of Wariness of Heights , 1992 .

[19]  Marion A. Eppler,et al.  Development of perception of affordances. , 1993 .

[20]  Bennett I. Bertenthal,et al.  An Epigenetic Perspective on the Development of Self-Produced Locomotion and Its Consequences , 1994 .

[21]  C. Moore,et al.  Joint attention : its origins and role in development , 1995 .

[22]  M. Tomasello Joint attention as social cognition. , 1995 .

[23]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  A Dynamic Systems Approach to the Development of Cognition and Action , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[24]  M. Appelbaum,et al.  Affective reorganization in the infant, the mother, and the dyad: the role of upright locomotion and its timing. , 1995, Child development.

[25]  Marion A. Eppler,et al.  Exploration in the service of prospective control , 2000 .

[26]  David I. Anderson,et al.  Travel Broadens the Mind. , 2000, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[27]  K. Adolph Specificity of Learning: Why Infants Fall Over a Veritable Cliff , 2000, Psychological science.

[28]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  The importance of eyes: how infants interpret adult looking behavior. , 2002, Developmental psychology.

[29]  Melissa W. Clearfield,et al.  The role of crawling and walking experience in infant spatial memory. , 2004, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[30]  David I. Anderson,et al.  Avoidance of Heights on the Visual Cliff in Newly Walking Infants. , 2005, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[31]  Esther Thelen,et al.  Dynamic Systems Theories , 2007 .

[32]  A. Neal,et al.  Infant joint attention, temperament, and social competence in preschool children. , 2007, Child development.

[33]  Melissa W Clearfield,et al.  Learning by looking: Infants' social looking behavior across the transition from crawling to walking. , 2008, Journal of experimental child psychology.