What are infant siblings teaching us about autism in infancy?

International research to understand infant patterns of development in autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) has recently focused on a research paradigm involving prospective longitudinal studies of infant siblings of children with autism. Such designs use a comparison group of infant siblings without any familial risks (the low‐risk group) to gather longitudinal information about developmental skills across the first 3 years of life, followed by clinical diagnosis of ASD at 36 months. This review focuses on five topics: presence of ASD in the infant sibling groups, patterns and characteristics of motor development, patterns and characteristics of social and emotional development, patterns and characteristics of intentional communication, both verbal and nonverbal, and patterns that mark the onset of behaviors pathognomonic for ASD. Symptoms in all these areas typically begin to be detected during the age period of 12–24 months in infants who will develop autism. Onset of the symptoms occurs at varying ages and in varying patterns, but the pattern of frank loss of skills and marked regression reported from previous retrospective studies in 20–30% of children is seldom reported in these infant sibling prospective studies. Two surprises involve the very early onset of repetitive and unusual sensory behaviors, and the lack of predictive symptoms at the age of 6 months. Contrary to current views that autism is a disorder that profoundly affects social development from the earliest months of life, the data from these studies presents a picture of autism as a disorder involving symptoms across multiple domains with a gradual onset that changes both ongoing developmental rate and established behavioral patterns across the first 2–3 years of life.

[1]  L. Kanner Autistic disturbances of affective contact. , 1968, Acta paedopsychiatrica.

[2]  T. Brazelton,et al.  The infant's response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction. , 1978, Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry.

[3]  E. Thelen Rhythmical stereotypies in normal human infants , 1979, Animal Behaviour.

[4]  R. Aslin,et al.  Preference for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth. , 1990, Child development.

[5]  Ami Klin,et al.  Young autistic children's listening preferences in regard to speech: A possible characterization of the symptom of social withdrawal , 1991, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[6]  B. Garreau,et al.  Autism and family home movies: Preliminary findings , 1991, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[7]  C. Gillberg,et al.  Can Autism be Detected at 18 Months? , 1992, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[8]  G. Dawson,et al.  Early recognition of children with autism: A study of first birthday home videotapes , 1994, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[9]  A. Bailey,et al.  Autism: The Phenotype in Relatives , 1998, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[10]  G. Baranek Autism During Infancy: A Retrospective Video Analysis of Sensory-Motor and Social Behaviors at 9–12 Months of Age , 1999, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[11]  D. Skuse Endophenotypes and child psychiatry , 2001, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[12]  F. Volkmar,et al.  Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. , 2002, Archives of general psychiatry.

[13]  G. Dawson,et al.  Defining the broader phenotype of autism: Genetic, brain, and behavioral perspectives , 2002, Development and Psychopathology.

[14]  J. Constantino,et al.  Autistic traits in the general population: a twin study. , 2003, Archives of general psychiatry.

[15]  Patricia K. Kuhl,et al.  Fast Track , 2020, Electric and Hybrid Vehicle Technology International.

[16]  P. Kuhl,et al.  Speech perception in infancy predicts language development in the second year of life: a longitudinal study. , 2004, Child development.

[17]  Geraldine Dawson,et al.  Links between social and linguistic processing of speech in preschool children with autism: behavioral and electrophysiological measures. , 2005, Developmental science.

[18]  M. Spence,et al.  Brief Report: Early Social Communication Behaviors in the Younger Siblings of Children with Autism , 2005, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[19]  J. Saffran,et al.  Changing the tune: the structure of the input affects infants' use of absolute and relative pitch. , 2005, Developmental science.

[20]  J. Brian,et al.  Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of life , 2005, International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience.

[21]  R. Landa,et al.  Development in infants with autism spectrum disorders: a prospective study. , 2006, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[22]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  The development of siblings of children with autism at 4 and 14 months: social engagement, communication, and cognition. , 2006, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[23]  J. Iverson,et al.  Variation in Vocal-Motor Development in Infant Siblings of Children with Autism , 2007, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[24]  S. Ozonoff,et al.  The Very Early Autism Phenotype , 2007 .

[25]  E. Turiel The Development of Morality , 2007 .

[26]  S. Rogers,et al.  Visual Fixation Patterns during Reciprocal Social Interaction Distinguish a Subgroup of 6-Month-Old Infants At-Risk for Autism from Comparison Infants , 2007, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[27]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Infancy and autism: progress, prospects, and challenges. , 2007, Progress in brain research.

[28]  D. Messinger,et al.  Early Social and Emotional Communication in the Infant Siblings of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders: An Examination of the Broad Phenotype , 2007, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[29]  Elizabeth Garrett-Mayer,et al.  Social and communication development in toddlers with early and later diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders. , 2007, Archives of general psychiatry.

[30]  J. Brian,et al.  Stereotyped Motor Behaviors Associated with Autism in High-risk Infants: A Pilot Videotape Analysis of a Sibling Sample , 2007, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[31]  Joseph P. McCleery,et al.  Abnormal Magnocellular Pathway Visual Processing in Infants at Risk for Autism , 2007, Biological Psychiatry.

[32]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Early Social, Imitation, Play, and Language Abilities of Young Non-Autistic Siblings of Children with Autism , 2007, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[33]  W. Stone,et al.  Effects of Different Attentional Cues on Responding to Joint Attention in Younger Siblings of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders , 2007, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[34]  Agata Rozga,et al.  A prospective study of response to name in infants at risk for autism. , 2007, Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine.

[35]  Jessica Brian,et al.  A Prospective Case Series of High-risk Infants who Developed Autism , 2007, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[36]  P. Mundy,et al.  Individual differences and the development of joint attention in infancy. , 2007, Child development.

[37]  R. Landa,et al.  Response to Joint Attention in Toddlers at Risk for Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Prospective Study , 2007, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[38]  W. Stone,et al.  Early social-communicative and cognitive development of younger siblings of children with autism spectrum disorders. , 2007, Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine.

[39]  M. Sigman,et al.  The Development of Young Siblings of Children with Autism from 4 to 54 Months , 2007, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[40]  Jessica Brian,et al.  The Autism Observation Scale for Infants: Scale Development and Reliability Data , 2008, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[41]  Sally J. Rogers,et al.  The Early Start Denver Model , 2008 .

[42]  Mark Sheskin,et al.  Visual disengagement in the infant siblings of children with an autism spectrum disorder (ASD) , 2008, Autism : the international journal of research and practice.

[43]  W. Roberts,et al.  Clinical assessment of autism in high-risk 18-month-olds , 2008, Autism : the international journal of research and practice.

[44]  J. Kagan In defense of qualitative changes in development. , 2008, Child development.

[45]  I. Hertz-Picciotto,et al.  The onset of autism: patterns of symptom emergence in the first years of life , 2008, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.

[46]  Sally Ozonoff,et al.  Atypical object exploration at 12 months of age is associated with autism in a prospective sample , 2008, Autism : the international journal of research and practice.

[47]  Tony Charman,et al.  Visual orienting in the early broader autism phenotype: disengagement and facilitation. , 2009, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[48]  W. Stone,et al.  Predicting Social Impairment and ASD Diagnosis in Younger Siblings of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2009, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[49]  Tony Charman,et al.  Neural Correlates of Eye Gaze Processing in the Infant Broader Autism Phenotype , 2009, Biological Psychiatry.

[50]  Jessica Brian,et al.  Temperament and its Relationship to Autistic Symptoms in a High-Risk Infant Sib Cohort , 2009, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[51]  S. Rogers,et al.  Gaze behavior and affect at 6 months: predicting clinical outcomes and language development in typically developing infants and infants at risk for autism. , 2009, Developmental science.