Salience and Default Mode Network Coupling Predicts Cognition in Aging and Parkinson’s Disease

Abstract Objectives: Cognitive impairment is common in Parkinson’s disease (PD). Three neurocognitive networks support efficient cognition: the salience network, the default mode network, and the central executive network. The salience network is thought to switch between activating and deactivating the default mode and central executive networks. Anti-correlated interactions between the salience and default mode networks in particular are necessary for efficient cognition. Our previous work demonstrated altered functional coupling between the neurocognitive networks in non-demented individuals with PD compared to age-matched control participants. Here, we aim to identify associations between cognition and functional coupling between these neurocognitive networks in the same group of participants. Methods: We investigated the extent to which intrinsic functional coupling among these neurocognitive networks is related to cognitive performance across three neuropsychological domains: executive functioning, psychomotor speed, and verbal memory. Twenty-four non-demented individuals with mild to moderate PD and 20 control participants were scanned at rest and evaluated on three neuropsychological domains. Results: PD participants were impaired on tests from all three domains compared to control participants. Our imaging results demonstrated that successful cognition across healthy aging and Parkinson’s disease participants was related to anti-correlated coupling between the salience and default mode networks. Individuals with poorer performance scores across groups demonstrated more positive salience network/default-mode network coupling. Conclusions: Successful cognition relies on healthy coupling between the salience and default mode networks, which may become dysfunctional in PD. These results can help inform non-pharmacological interventions (repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation) targeting these specific networks before they become vulnerable in early stages of Parkinson’s disease. (JINS, 2016, 22, 205–215)

[1]  Q. Gong,et al.  Dysfunction of the Default Mode Network in Drug-Naïve Parkinson’s Disease with Mild Cognitive Impairments: A Resting-State fMRI Study , 2016, Front. Aging Neurosci..

[2]  S. Houle,et al.  Salience network and parahippocampal dopamine dysfunction in memory‐impaired Parkinson disease , 2015, Annals of neurology.

[3]  C. Stern,et al.  Altered intrinsic functional coupling between core neurocognitive networks in Parkinson's disease , 2015, NeuroImage: Clinical.

[4]  M. Hallett,et al.  Deficits in task-set maintenance and execution networks in Parkinson’s disease , 2016, Brain Structure and Function.

[5]  Robert Leech,et al.  Damage to the Salience Network and Interactions with the Default Mode Network , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  Antonio P Strafella,et al.  Uncovering the role of the insula in non-motor symptoms of Parkinson's disease. , 2014, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[7]  L. Nickerson,et al.  An Increase in Tobacco Craving Is Associated with Enhanced Medial Prefrontal Cortex Network Coupling , 2014, PloS one.

[8]  S. Houle,et al.  Combined insular and striatal dopamine dysfunction are associated with executive deficits in Parkinson's disease with mild cognitive impairment. , 2014, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[9]  K A Sigvardt,et al.  Resting state functional connectivity is associated with cognitive dysfunction in non-demented people with Parkinson's disease. , 2014, Journal of Parkinson's disease.

[10]  D. Sharp,et al.  The role of the posterior cingulate cortex in cognition and disease. , 2014, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  Daniël Lakens,et al.  Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: a practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[12]  M. Jahanshahi,et al.  Executive dysfunction in Parkinson's disease: a review. , 2013, Journal of neuropsychology.

[13]  A. Moustafa,et al.  Factors underlying probabilistic and deterministic stimulus-response learning performance in medicated and unmedicated patients with Parkinson's disease. , 2013, Neuropsychology.

[14]  Mark D'Esposito,et al.  The salience network causally influences default mode network activity during moral reasoning. , 2013, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[15]  U. Bonuccelli,et al.  Acute and chronic cognitive effects of levodopa and dopamine agonists on patients with Parkinson’s disease: a review , 2013, Therapeutic advances in psychopharmacology.

[16]  C. Stern,et al.  Frontostriatal and mediotemporal lobe contributions to implicit higher-order spatial sequence learning declines in aging and Parkinson's disease. , 2013, Behavioral neuroscience.

[17]  Megan M. Risi,et al.  Frontal and posterior subtypes of neuropsychological deficit in Parkinson's disease. , 2013, Behavioral neuroscience.

[18]  N. Narayanan,et al.  Prefrontal dopamine signaling and cognitive symptoms of Parkinson’s disease , 2013, Reviews in the neurosciences.

[19]  Fabrizio Esposito,et al.  Default-mode network connectivity in cognitively unimpaired patients with Parkinson disease , 2012, Neurology.

[20]  C. Tanner,et al.  Dopamine transporter imaging is associated with long‐term outcomes in Parkinson's disease , 2012, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[21]  Wei Liao,et al.  Large-Scale Brain Networks in Board Game Experts: Insights from a Domain-Related Task and Task-Free Resting State , 2012, PloS one.

[22]  Robert Leech,et al.  Salience network integrity predicts default mode network function after traumatic brain injury , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[23]  A. Strafella,et al.  Levodopa influences striatal activity but does not affect cortical hyper‐activity in Parkinson’s disease , 2012, The European journal of neuroscience.

[24]  M. Mikl,et al.  The default mode network integrity in patients with Parkinson’s disease is levodopa equivalent dose-dependent , 2012, Journal of Neural Transmission.

[25]  J. Kulisevsky,et al.  Cognitive impairment in nondemented Parkinson's disease , 2011, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[26]  Jessica A. Turner,et al.  Behavioral Interpretations of Intrinsic Connectivity Networks , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  L. Clare,et al.  Executive functions in Parkinson's disease: Systematic review and meta‐analysis , 2011, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[28]  V. Menon Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: a unifying triple network model , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[29]  D. Aarsland,et al.  Verbal memory in drug-naive, newly diagnosed Parkinson's disease. The retrieval deficit hypothesis revisited. , 2011, Neuropsychology.

[30]  Paul M. Matthews,et al.  Nicotine replacement in abstinent smokers improves cognitive withdrawal symptoms with modulation of resting brain network dynamics , 2010, NeuroImage.

[31]  Zhishun Wang,et al.  Visual inspection of independent components: Defining a procedure for artifact removal from fMRI data , 2010, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[32]  S. Bressler,et al.  Large-scale brain networks in cognition: emerging methods and principles , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[33]  V. Menon,et al.  Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function , 2010, Brain Structure and Function.

[34]  S. Anderson,et al.  Incidence of and risk factors for cognitive impairment in an early Parkinson disease clinical trial cohort , 2009, Neurology.

[35]  Thomas Foltynie,et al.  The distinct cognitive syndromes of Parkinson's disease: 5 year follow-up of the CamPaIGN cohort. , 2009, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[36]  N. Filippini,et al.  Distinct patterns of brain activity in young carriers of the APOE e4 allele , 2009, NeuroImage.

[37]  Oury Monchi,et al.  Dysfunction of the default mode network in Parkinson disease: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. , 2009, Archives of neurology.

[38]  N. Filippini,et al.  Group comparison of resting-state FMRI data using multi-subject ICA and dual regression , 2009, NeuroImage.

[39]  Daniel Weintraub,et al.  Mild cognitive impairment is common in Parkinson's disease patients with normal Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) scores. , 2009, Parkinsonism & related disorders.

[40]  R. Nathan Spreng,et al.  The Common Neural Basis of Autobiographical Memory, Prospection, Navigation, Theory of Mind, and the Default Mode: A Quantitative Meta-analysis , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[41]  Peter Fransson,et al.  The precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex plays a pivotal role in the default mode network: Evidence from a partial correlation network analysis , 2008, NeuroImage.

[42]  V. Menon,et al.  A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[43]  Chantal E. Stern,et al.  Fronto-striatal deficit in Parkinson's disease during semantic event sequencing , 2008, Neurobiology of Aging.

[44]  T. Babić,et al.  Is quality of life in non‐demented Parkinson’s disease patients related to cognitive performance? A clinic‐based cross‐sectional study , 2008, European journal of neurology.

[45]  Bharat B. Biswal,et al.  Competition between functional brain networks mediates behavioral variability , 2008, NeuroImage.

[46]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Intrinsic Fluctuations within Cortical Systems Account for Intertrial Variability in Human Behavior , 2007, Neuron.

[47]  G. Glover,et al.  Dissociable Intrinsic Connectivity Networks for Salience Processing and Executive Control , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  David Eidelberg,et al.  Metabolic brain networks associated with cognitive function in Parkinson's disease , 2007, NeuroImage.

[49]  M. Petrides,et al.  Cortical activity in Parkinson's disease during executive processing depends on striatal involvement. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[50]  A. Dagher,et al.  Basal ganglia functional connectivity based on a meta-analysis of 126 positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging publications. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[51]  K. Hugdahl,et al.  Subtypes of mild cognitive impairment in parkinson's disease: Progression to dementia , 2006, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[52]  Kristina M. Visscher,et al.  The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[53]  Kristina M. Visscher,et al.  A Core System for the Implementation of Task Sets , 2006, Neuron.

[54]  T. Robbins,et al.  Dopaminergic basis for deficits in working memory but not attentional set-shifting in Parkinson's disease , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[55]  J D Dawson,et al.  Visual dysfunction in Parkinson disease without dementia , 2005, Neurology.

[56]  J. Fudge,et al.  Insular and gustatory inputs to the caudal ventral striatum in primates , 2005, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[57]  Maurizio Corbetta,et al.  The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[58]  Angela R Laird,et al.  Brainmap taxonomy of experimental design: Description and evaluation , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[59]  Angela R. Laird,et al.  BrainMap , 2007, Neuroinformatics.

[60]  E. Wolters,et al.  Cognitive dysfunction and dementia in Parkinson’s disease , 2004, Journal of Neural Transmission.

[61]  T. Robbins,et al.  The cognitive ability of an incident cohort of Parkinson's patients in the UK. The CamPaIGN study. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[62]  T. Tombaugh Trail Making Test A and B: normative data stratified by age and education. , 2004, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[63]  Roshan Cools,et al.  Using executive heterogeneity to explore the nature of working memory deficits in Parkinson’s disease , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[64]  L. K. Hansen,et al.  Independent component analysis of functional MRI: what is signal and what is noise? , 2003, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[65]  T. Robbins,et al.  Cognitive Impairments in Early Parkinson's Disease Are Accompanied by Reductions in Activity in Frontostriatal Neural Circuitry , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[66]  D. Aarsland,et al.  Prevalence and characteristics of dementia in Parkinson disease: an 8-year prospective study. , 2003, Archives of neurology.

[67]  Kenneth Hugdahl,et al.  Neuropsychological Profile of Patients with Parkinson’s Disease without Dementia , 2003, Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders.

[68]  Vinod Menon,et al.  Functional connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[69]  K. Kortte,et al.  The Trail Making Test, Part B: Cognitive Flexibility or Ability to Maintain Set? , 2002, Applied neuropsychology.

[70]  M. Carlier,et al.  Measuring handedness: A validation study of Bishop's reaching card test , 2002, Laterality.

[71]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[72]  L A Buddenberg,et al.  Test-retest reliability of the Purdue Pegboard Test. , 2000, The American journal of occupational therapy : official publication of the American Occupational Therapy Association.

[73]  P. Strick,et al.  Basal ganglia and cerebellar loops: motor and cognitive circuits , 2000, Brain Research Reviews.

[74]  I. Grant,et al.  Test–retest reliability and practice effects of Expanded Halstead–Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery , 1999, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[75]  Suzanne N. Haber,et al.  Insular Cortical Projections to Functional Regions of the Striatum Correlate with Cortical Cytoarchitectonic Organization in the Primate , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[76]  P. Satz,et al.  Performance of four age groups of normal elderly on the Rey Auditory-Verbal Learning Test. , 1991, Journal of clinical psychology.

[77]  O. Spreen,et al.  A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary , 1991 .

[78]  S. Kish,et al.  Uneven pattern of dopamine loss in the striatum of patients with idiopathic Parkinson's disease. Pathophysiologic and clinical implications. , 1988, The New England journal of medicine.

[79]  Virgil G Mathiowetz,et al.  The Purdue Pegboard , 1986 .

[80]  P. Rubé,et al.  L’examen Clinique en Psychologie , 1959 .

[81]  J. Tiffin,et al.  The Purdue pegboard; norms and studies of reliability and validity. , 1948, The Journal of applied psychology.