The role of local grouping and global orientation contrast in perception of orientation-modulated textures

We explored the contribution to perception of orientation-modulated textures of visual processes selective either for orientation contrast or orientation grouping. To distinguish between these two processes we manipulated the axis of local grouping of texture elements independently of the direction of global orientation modulation. The general question posed was whether visibility of texture structure (measured as threshold for discriminating spatial-frequency of texture structure) is dependent on the magnitude of orientation contrast, strength and direction of local grouping, or some combination of the two. We demonstrated that the factor of primary importance is the amplitude of global orientation contrast rather than the presence of local grouping content. Using orientation-interleaved textures (containing two superimposed textures modulated around orthogonal orientations), we further showed that orientation single-opponent processes are a more likely candidate for detecting orientation contrast than double-opponent processes.

[1]  W Singer,et al.  The Perceptual Grouping Criterion of Colinearity is Reflected by Anisotropies of Connections in the Primary Visual Cortex , 1997, The European journal of neuroscience.

[2]  C. Li,et al.  Extensive integration field beyond the classical receptive field of cat's striate cortical neurons--classification and tuning properties. , 1994, Vision research.

[3]  D. V. van Essen,et al.  Neuronal responses to static texture patterns in area V1 of the alert macaque monkey. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  H. Jones,et al.  Visual cortical mechanisms detecting focal orientation discontinuities , 1995, Nature.

[5]  J. Beck,et al.  Contrast and spatial variables in texture segregation: Testing a simple spatial-frequency channels model , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.

[6]  D. Levi,et al.  Vernier acuity with plaid masks: the role of oriented filters in vernier acuity , 1997, Vision Research.

[7]  J. Beck,et al.  Line segregation. , 1989, Spatial vision.

[8]  U. Polat,et al.  The architecture of perceptual spatial interactions , 1994, Vision Research.

[9]  A. Rosenfeld,et al.  A Theory of Textural Segmentation , 1983 .

[10]  C L Baker,et al.  A processing stream in mammalian visual cortex neurons for non-Fourier responses. , 1993, Science.

[11]  G. Sperling,et al.  Drift-balanced random stimuli: a general basis for studying non-Fourier motion perception. , 1988, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[12]  D Regan,et al.  Spatial frequency discrimination and detection characteristics for gratings defined by orientation texture , 1998, Vision Research.

[13]  G. Mitchison,et al.  Long axons within the striate cortex: their distribution, orientation, and patterns of connection. , 1982, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[14]  F. Kingdom,et al.  Sensitivity to orientation modulation in micropattern-based textures , 1995, Vision Research.

[15]  Vision Research , 1961, Nature.

[16]  B Moulden,et al.  Collator units: second-stage orientational filters. , 1994, Ciba Foundation symposium.

[17]  Azriel Rosenfeld,et al.  Human and Machine Vision , 1983 .

[18]  J. Robson,et al.  Application of fourier analysis to the visibility of gratings , 1968, The Journal of physiology.

[19]  Orientation- and frequency-modulated textures at low depths of modulation are processed by off-orientation and off-frequency texture mechanisms , 2002, Vision Research.

[20]  S Grossberg,et al.  Neural dynamics of perceptual grouping: Textures, boundaries, and emergent segmentations , 1985, Perception & psychophysics.

[21]  Gregory Bock,et al.  Higher-order processing in the visual system , 1994 .

[22]  H. Barlow Retinal noise and absolute threshold. , 1956, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[23]  A Pentland,et al.  Maximum likelihood estimation: The best PEST , 1980, Perception & psychophysics.

[24]  F. Kingdom,et al.  Luminance spatial frequency differences facilitate the segmentation of superimposed textures , 2000, Vision Research.

[25]  J. Lund,et al.  Intrinsic laminar lattice connections in primate visual cortex , 1983, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[26]  A. Mussap,et al.  Alignment of orientation-modulated textures , 2000, Vision Research.

[27]  P Perona,et al.  Preattentive texture discrimination with early vision mechanisms. , 1990, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[28]  Victor A. F. Lamme,et al.  Contextual Modulation in Primary Visual Cortex , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  J. Smits,et al.  The perception of a dotted line in noise: a model of good continuation and some experimental results. , 1985, Spatial vision.

[30]  G. Sperling,et al.  Measuring the spatial frequency selectivity of second-order texture mechanisms , 1995, Vision Research.

[31]  B. S. Rubenstein,et al.  Effects of foreground scale in texture discrimination tasks: performance is size, shape, and content specific. , 1993, Spatial vision.

[32]  Wilson S. Geisler,et al.  Multichannel Texture Analysis Using Localized Spatial Filters , 1990, IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell..

[33]  U. Polat,et al.  Lateral interactions between spatial channels: Suppression and facilitation revealed by lateral masking experiments , 1993, Vision Research.

[34]  J J Knierim,et al.  Neural responses to visual texture patterns in middle temporal area of the macaque monkey. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  A J Mussap,et al.  Adaptation Reveals a Neural Code for the Visual Location of Orientation Change , 2001, Perception.

[36]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex , 1962, The Journal of physiology.

[37]  F. Kingdom,et al.  A linear systems approach to the detection of both abrupt and smooth spatial variations in orientation-defined textures , 1996, Vision Research.

[38]  Horace B. Barlow,et al.  Detecting collinear dots in noise , 1999, Vision Research.

[39]  R. von der Heydt,et al.  Periodic-pattern-selective cells in monkey visual cortex , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[40]  M. Morgan,et al.  Perceived diagonals in grids and lattices , 1989, Vision Research.

[41]  F. Kingdom,et al.  Modulation frequency and orientation tuning of second-order texture mechanisms. , 1999, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[42]  H. Nothdurft Feature analysis and the role of similarity in preattentive vision , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[43]  Jacob Beck,et al.  Spatial frequency channels and perceptual grouping in texture segregation , 1987, Comput. Vis. Graph. Image Process..

[44]  David J. Field,et al.  Contour integration by the human visual system: Evidence for a local “association field” , 1993, Vision Research.

[45]  D. Pelli The quantum efficiency of vision , 1990 .

[46]  N. Graham,et al.  Spatial-frequency- and orientation-selectivity of simple and complex channels in region segregation , 1993, Vision Research.