Transfer of motor performance in an obstacle avoidance task to different walking conditions.

The aim of this study was to examine whether subjects who have learned a skilled locomotor task can transfer the acquired performance to conditions involving either a change in the external coordinates or in the sensory input from one leg. Subjects were trained to step over an obstacle with minimal foot clearance without visual information about either the obstacle or their legs during treadmill walking. Leg muscle activity and joint kinematics were recorded and analyzed. Acoustic signals provided feedback about foot clearance over the obstacle. After successful training, the transfer of learning between level and downhill walking and to walking with additional weight attached to the leg was examined. It was found that once subjects learned to step over the obstacle at an optimal foot clearance, they could transfer their performance within the first step over the obstacle in the new walking conditions. Closer examination of the transfer between level and downhill walking revealed no consistent kinematic strategy across subjects. To transfer the learned performance to walking with additional weight, subjects consistently and automatically increased biceps femoris muscle activation. The results are discussed in the context of emerging concepts in the neural control of walking and motor learning.

[1]  F B Horak,et al.  Forward versus backward walking: transfer of podokinetic adaptation. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[3]  J. Duysens,et al.  Significance of load receptor input during locomotion: a review. , 2000, Gait & posture.

[4]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Viewing the hand prior to movement improves accuracy of pointing performed toward the unseen contralateral hand , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[5]  T. Drew,et al.  Role of the motor cortex in the control of visually triggered gait modifications. , 1996, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[6]  F. Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  The motor system does not learn the dynamics of the arm by rote memorization of past experience. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Motor patterns for human gait: backward versus forward locomotion. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  D. Winter Foot trajectory in human gait: a precise and multifactorial motor control task. , 1992, Physical therapy.

[9]  Michel Desmurget,et al.  The effect of viewing the static hand prior to movement onset on pointing kinematics and variability , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  V. Dietz Proprioception and locomotor disorders , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[11]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Motor Patterns in Walking. , 1999, News in physiological sciences : an international journal of physiology produced jointly by the International Union of Physiological Sciences and the American Physiological Society.

[12]  R. Poppele,et al.  Proprioception from a spinocerebellar perspective. , 2001, Physiological reviews.

[13]  Aftab E. Patla,et al.  Review article Understanding the roles of vision in the control of human locomotion , 1997 .

[14]  Keir G. Pearson,et al.  Sartorius muscle afferents influence the amplitude and timing of flexor activity in walking decerebrate cats , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Control of foot trajectory in human locomotion: role of ground contact forces in simulated reduced gravity. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  Daniel M Wolpert,et al.  Role of uncertainty in sensorimotor control. , 2002, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[17]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Learned dynamics of reaching movements generalize from dominant to nondominant arm. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  Amy J. Bastian,et al.  Inter- and intra-limb generalization of adaptation during catching , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  D A McCrea,et al.  Group I disynaptic excitation of cat hindlimb flexor and bifunctional motoneurones during fictive locomotion , 2000, The Journal of physiology.

[20]  R Shadmehr,et al.  Electromyographic Correlates of Learning an Internal Model of Reaching Movements , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  H. Barbeau,et al.  Postural adaptation to walking on inclined surfaces: I. Normal strategies. , 2002, Gait & posture.

[22]  C. R. Gordon,et al.  Adaptive plasticity in the control of locomotor trajectory , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  J. Gordon,et al.  Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. II. Effects of visual information on accuracy. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  Gammon M Earhart,et al.  Podokinetic after-rotation following unilateral and bilateral podokinetic stimulation. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  R F Reynolds,et al.  The moving platform aftereffect: limited generalization of a locomotor adaptation. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  Gammon M Earhart,et al.  Transfer of podokinetic adaptation from stepping to hopping. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  J. Coast Handbook of Physiology. Section 12. Exercise: Regulation and Integration of Multiple Systems , 1997 .

[28]  K G Pearson,et al.  Neural adaptation in the generation of rhythmic behavior. , 2000, Annual review of physiology.

[29]  R Shadmehr,et al.  Spatial Generalization from Learning Dynamics of Reaching Movements , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  E Bizzi,et al.  Motor learning by field approximation. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[31]  V. Dietz,et al.  Obstacle avoidance during human walking: transfer of motor skill from one leg to the other , 2002, The Journal of physiology.

[32]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Short-Term Memory for Reaching to Visual Targets: Psychophysical Evidence for Body-Centered Reference Frames , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  Lori Ann Vallis,et al.  Locomotor adjustments for circumvention of an obstacle in the travel path , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[34]  D. McCrea Spinal circuitry of sensorimotor control of locomotion , 2001, The Journal of physiology.

[35]  V. Dietz,et al.  Obstacle avoidance during human walking: learning rate and cross‐modal transfer , 2001, The Journal of physiology.

[36]  W. Zijlstra,et al.  Adaptational and learning processes during human split-belt locomotion: interaction between central mechanisms and afferent input , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[37]  M. Sirota,et al.  Integration of motor and visual information in the parietal area 5 during locomotion. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  J R Flanagan,et al.  Composition and Decomposition of Internal Models in Motor Learning under Altered Kinematic and Dynamic Environments , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Interactions between posture and locomotion: motor patterns in humans walking with bent posture versus erect posture. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  Stuart Anstis,et al.  Aftereffects from jogging , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[41]  David A. Winter,et al.  Biomechanics and Motor Control of Human Movement , 1990 .

[42]  H. Barbeau,et al.  Adaptation of the walking pattern to uphill walking in normal and spinal-cord injured subjects , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[43]  M. Kuster,et al.  Kinematic and kinetic comparison of downhill and level walking. , 1995, Clinical biomechanics.

[44]  R. Poppele,et al.  Reference frames for spinal proprioception: limb endpoint based or joint-level based? , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.