Mapping the intersection of language and reading: the neural bases of the primary systems hypothesis

The primary systems framework has been used to relate behavioural performance across many different language activities to the status of core underpinning domain-general cognitive systems. This study provided the first quantitative investigation of this account at both behavioural and neural levels in a group of patients with chronic post-stroke aphasia. Principal components analysis was used to distil orthogonal measures of phonological and semantic processing, which were then related to reading performance and the underlying lesion distributions using voxel-based correlational methodology. Concrete word reading involved both a ventral semantic pathway, and inferior and anterior aspects of the dorsal phonological pathway. Abstract word reading overlapped with the ventral semantic pathway but also drew more extensively on the superior and posterior aspects of the dorsal phonological pathway. Nonword reading was related to phonological processing along the dorsal pathway and was also supported by a more superior set of regions previously associated with speech motor output. The use of continuous measures of behavioural performance and neural integrity allowed us to elucidate for the first time both the lesion and behavioural correlates for the semantic and phonological components of the primary systems hypothesis and to extend these by identifying the importance of an additional dorsal speech motor output system. These results provide a target for future neuroanatomically constrained computational models of reading.

[1]  M. L. Lambon Ralph,et al.  The role of plasticity-related functional reorganization in the explanation of central dyslexias , 2011, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[2]  Elizabeth Jefferies,et al.  Semantic Processing in the Anterior Temporal Lobes: A Meta-analysis of the Functional Neuroimaging Literature , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[3]  David A. Medler,et al.  Distinct Brain Systems for Processing Concrete and Abstract Concepts , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[4]  Bernard Mazoyer,et al.  Meta-analyzing left hemisphere language areas: Phonology, semantics, and sentence processing , 2006, NeuroImage.

[5]  Richard J. Binney,et al.  Differing contributions of inferior prefrontal and anterior temporal cortex to concrete and abstract conceptual knowledge , 2015, Cortex.

[6]  Jesper Andersson,et al.  Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic , 2005, NeuroImage.

[7]  Paul Hoffman,et al.  Triangulation of the neurocomputational architecture underpinning reading aloud , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[8]  M. Schwartz,et al.  The dorsal stream contribution to phonological retrieval in object naming. , 2012, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[9]  Maya L. Henry,et al.  Phonological dyslexia and dysgraphia: Cognitive mechanisms and neural substrates , 2009, Cortex.

[10]  R. Nass,et al.  The assessment of aphasia and related disorders By Harold Goodglass and edith kaplan philadelphia, lea & febiger, 1983 illustrated, $27.50 (package) , 1984 .

[11]  Ajay D. Halai,et al.  Using principal component analysis to capture individual differences within a unified neuropsychological model of chronic post-stroke aphasia: Revealing the unique neural correlates of speech fluency, phonology and semantics , 2017, Cortex.

[12]  M. Catani,et al.  A novel frontal pathway underlies verbal fluency in primary progressive aphasia. , 2013, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[13]  M. L. Lambon Ralph,et al.  Homogeneity and heterogeneity in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease: a cross-sectional and longitudinal study of 55 cases. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[14]  Janice Kay,et al.  Phonological dyslexia and phonological impairment: An exception to the rule? , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  A. Woollams,et al.  When does less yield more? The impact of severity upon implicit recognition in pure alexia , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. , 1996, Psychological review.

[17]  Anil F. Ramlackhansingh,et al.  Lesion identification using unified segmentation-normalisation models and fuzzy clustering , 2008, NeuroImage.

[18]  Chris Westbury,et al.  Imageability x phonology interactions during lexical access: Effects of modality, phonological neighbourhood, and phonological processing efficiency , 2009 .

[19]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: A primer with examples , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[20]  Jeremy D. Schmahmann,et al.  Disconnection syndromes of basal ganglia, thalamus, and cerebrocerebellar systems , 2008, Cortex.

[21]  D. Plaut,et al.  SD-squared: on the association between semantic dementia and surface dyslexia. , 2007, Psychological review.

[22]  Luise Springer,et al.  The Comprehensive Aphasia Test: A review. Commentary on Howard, Swinburn, and Porter, “Putting the CAT out: What the Comprehensive Aphasia Test has to offer” , 2010 .

[23]  A. Hillis,et al.  The roles of occipitotemporal cortex in reading, spelling, and naming , 2014, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[24]  J. Tree Two types of phonological dyslexia – A contemporary review , 2008, Cortex.

[25]  F. Molteni,et al.  The anatomical foundations of acquired reading disorders: A neuropsychological verification of the dual-route model of reading , 2014, Brain and Language.

[26]  Geoffrey A. Donnan,et al.  A Digital Map of Middle Cerebral Artery Infarcts Associated With Middle Cerebral Artery Trunk and Branch Occlusion , 2005, Stroke.

[27]  H. Duffau,et al.  Role of fronto-striatal tract and frontal aslant tract in movement and speech: an axonal mapping study , 2014, Brain Structure and Function.

[28]  J. Hodges,et al.  Non-verbal semantic impairment in semantic dementia , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  Chris Rorden,et al.  Regional White Matter Damage Predicts Speech Fluency in Chronic Post-Stroke Aphasia , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[30]  Chris Rorden,et al.  Re-establishing Broca’s initial findings , 2012, Brain and Language.

[31]  D. Plaut,et al.  SD-squared revisited: reply to Coltheart, Tree, and Saunders (2010). , 2010, Psychological review.

[32]  Oren Civier,et al.  The frontal aslant tract underlies speech fluency in persistent developmental stuttering , 2014, Brain Structure and Function.

[33]  Kim S. Graham,et al.  One bird with two stones: Abnormal word length effects in pure alexia and semantic dementia , 2006, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[34]  Derek K. Jones,et al.  Perisylvian language networks of the human brain , 2005, Annals of neurology.

[35]  T. Rogers,et al.  Lichtheim 2: Synthesizing Aphasia and the Neural Basis of Language in a Neurocomputational Model of the Dual Dorsal-Ventral Language Pathways , 2011, Neuron.

[36]  Régine Kolinsky,et al.  How Early Does the Brain Distinguish between Regular Words, Irregular Words, and Pseudowords during the Reading Process? Evidence from Neurochronometric TMS , 2015, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[37]  Volkmar Glauche,et al.  Ventral and dorsal pathways for language , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[38]  Nathalie Tzourio-Mazoyer,et al.  New insights into the anatomo-functional connectivity of the semantic system: a study using cortico-subcortical electrostimulations. , 2005, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[39]  Pascale Tremblay,et al.  The Language Connectome , 2014, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[40]  Cathy J. Price,et al.  A review and synthesis of the first 20 years of PET and fMRI studies of heard speech, spoken language and reading , 2012, NeuroImage.

[41]  Lorraine K. Tyler,et al.  Dissociating neuro-cognitive component processes: voxel-based correlational methodology , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[42]  Daniel Mirman,et al.  The Neural Basis of Inhibitory Effects of Semantic and Phonological Neighbors in Spoken Word Production , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[43]  Gene E. Alexander,et al.  Written Language Impairments in Primary Progressive Aphasia: A Reflection of Damage to Central Semantic and Phonological Processes , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[44]  B. Miller,et al.  The neural basis of surface dyslexia in semantic dementia. , 2009, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[45]  M. Catani,et al.  The rises and falls of disconnection syndromes. , 2005, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[46]  Steven Z. Rapcsak,et al.  Efficient Visual Object and Word Recognition Relies on High Spatial Frequency Coding in the Left Posterior Fusiform Gyrus: Evidence from a Case-Series of Patients with Ventral Occipito-Temporal Cortex Damage , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[47]  M. A. Lambon Ralph,et al.  The inferior, anterior temporal lobes and semantic memory clarified: Novel evidence from distortion-corrected fMRI , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[48]  F. Vassal,et al.  New insights into the functional significance of the frontal aslant tract: An anatomo–functional study using intraoperative electrical stimulations combined with diffusion tensor imaging-based fiber tracking , 2014, British journal of neurosurgery.

[49]  S. Nadeau,et al.  Frequency, nature, and predictors of alexia in a convenience sample of individuals with chronic aphasia , 2014 .

[50]  Richard J. Binney,et al.  The ventral and inferolateral aspects of the anterior temporal lobe are crucial in semantic memory: evidence from a novel direct comparison of distortion-corrected fMRI, rTMS, and semantic dementia. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[51]  Allan Paivio,et al.  Dual coding theory and the mental lexicon , 2010 .

[52]  G. Porter,et al.  The Comprehensive Aphasia Test , 2005 .

[53]  Anna M. Woollams,et al.  Capturing multidimensionality in stroke aphasia: mapping principal behavioural components to neural structures , 2014, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[54]  M. Schlossberg The Assessment of Aphasia and Related Disorders. 2nd ed. , 1984 .

[55]  E. Kaplan,et al.  The assessment of aphasia and related disorders , 1972 .

[56]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. , 1989, Psychological review.

[57]  Matthew A. Lambon Ralph,et al.  Unlocking the Nature of the Phonological–Deep Dyslexia Continuum: The Keys to Reading Aloud Are in Phonology and Semantics , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[58]  Tim Shallice,et al.  The Hayling and Brixton Tests , 1997 .

[59]  Matthew A. Lambon Ralph,et al.  Lateralization of ventral and dorsal auditory-language pathways in the human brain , 2005, NeuroImage.

[60]  E. Kaplan,et al.  The Boston naming test , 2001 .

[61]  William W. Graves,et al.  Neurally dissociable cognitive components of reading deficits in subacute stroke , 2015, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[62]  Marko Wilke,et al.  Manual, semi-automated, and automated delineation of chronic brain lesions: A comparison of methods , 2011, NeuroImage.

[63]  Jeffrey R. Binder,et al.  Modulation of the semantic system by word imageability , 2005, NeuroImage.

[64]  F. Dick,et al.  Voxel-based lesion–symptom mapping , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[65]  Roy W Jones,et al.  Comprehension of concrete and abstract words in semantic dementia. , 2009, Neuropsychology.

[66]  Marco Catani,et al.  What is a disconnection syndrome? , 2008, Cortex.

[67]  R. Wise,et al.  Language systems in normal and aphasic human subjects: functional imaging studies and inferences from animal studies. , 2003, British medical bulletin.

[68]  Eraldo Paulesu,et al.  Reading the reading brain: A new meta-analysis of functional imaging data on reading , 2013, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[69]  T. Shallice,et al.  Deep Dyslexia: A Case Study of , 1993 .

[70]  Y. Hayashi,et al.  Intraoperative subcortical mapping of a language-associated deep frontal tract connecting the superior frontal gyrus to Broca's area in the dominant hemisphere of patients with glioma. , 2015, Journal of neurosurgery.

[71]  Matthew A. Lambon Ralph,et al.  Anomia is simply a reflection of semantic and phonological impairments: Evidence from a case-series study , 2002 .

[72]  Simona Maria Brambati,et al.  The role of the left anterior temporal lobe in exception word reading: Reconciling patient and neuroimaging findings , 2012, NeuroImage.

[73]  Karalyn Patterson,et al.  Selective disorders of reading? , 1999, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[74]  A. Woollams,et al.  Connectionist neuropsychology: uncovering ultimate causes of acquired dyslexia , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[75]  Joseph T. Devlin,et al.  Inferior Parietal Lobule Contributions to Visual Word Recognition , 2015, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[76]  Matthew H. Davis,et al.  Can cognitive models explain brain activation during word and pseudoword reading? A meta-analysis of 36 neuroimaging studies. , 2013, Psychological bulletin.

[77]  D. Plaut,et al.  Complementary neural representations for faces and words: A computational exploration , 2011, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[78]  E. Mandonnet,et al.  Is the left uncinate fasciculus essential for language? , 2009, Journal of Neurology.

[79]  Matthew A. Lambon Ralph,et al.  Using Parallel Distributed Processing Models to Simulate Phonological Dyslexia: The Key Role of Plasticity-related Recovery , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[80]  Max Coltheart,et al.  Psycholinguistic assessments of language processing in aphasia (PALPA) , 1996 .

[81]  J. Raven Coloured progressive matrices : sets A, Ab, B , 1956 .

[82]  Matthew A. Lambon Ralph,et al.  Unlocking the Nature of the Phonological–Deep Dyslexia Continuum: The Keys to Reading Aloud Are in Phonology and Semantics , 2006 .

[83]  Hanna Damasio,et al.  Specific Reading and Phonological Processing Deficits are Associated with Damage to the Left Frontal Operculum , 2006, Cortex.